Proximal Binaural Sound Can Induce Subjective Frisson

Auditory frisson is the experience of feeling of cold or shivering related to sound in the absence of a physical cold stimulus. Multiple examples of frisson-inducing sounds have been reported, but the mechanism of auditory frisson remains elusive. Typical frisson-inducing sounds may contain a looming effect, in which a sound appears to approach the listener's peripersonal space. Previous studies on sound in peripersonal space have provided objective measurements of sound-inducing effects, but few have investigated the subjective experience of frisson-inducing sounds. Here we explored whether it is possible to produce subjective feelings of frisson by moving a noise sound (white noise, rolling beads noise, or frictional noise produced by rubbing a plastic bag) stimulus around a listener's head. Our results demonstrated that sound-induced frisson can be experienced stronger when auditory stimuli are rotated around the head (binaural moving sounds) than the one without the rotation (monaural static sounds), regardless of the source of the noise sound. Pearson's correlation analysis showed that several acoustic features of auditory stimuli, such as variance of interaural level difference (ILD), loudness, and sharpness, were correlated with the magnitude of subjective frisson. We had also observed that the subjective feelings of frisson by moving a musical sound had increased comparing with a static musical sound.

[1]  R. Zatorre,et al.  Intensely pleasurable responses to music correlate with activity in brain regions implicated in reward and emotion , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[2]  Stephen D. Smith,et al.  An Examination of Personality Traits Associated with Autonomous Sensory Meridian Response (ASMR) , 2017, Front. Psychol..

[3]  W. M. Rabinowitz,et al.  Auditory localization of nearby sources. Head-related transfer functions. , 1999, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[4]  Kirk N. Olsen,et al.  A “looming bias” in spatial hearing? Effects of acoustic intensity and spectrum on categorical sound source localization , 2017, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[5]  Romke Rouw,et al.  Misophonia: physiological investigations and case descriptions , 2013, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[6]  Damiaan Denys,et al.  Misophonia: Diagnostic Criteria for a New Psychiatric Disorder , 2013, PloS one.

[7]  David Margulis,et al.  Musical expectancy and thrills , 2011 .

[8]  J. Sloboda Music Structure and Emotional Response: Some Empirical Findings , 1991 .

[9]  Ana Tajadura-Jiménez,et al.  Embodied auditory perception: the emotional impact of approaching and receding sound sources. , 2010, Emotion.

[10]  R. Zatorre,et al.  Anatomically distinct dopamine release during anticipation and experience of peak emotion to music , 2011, Nature Neuroscience.

[11]  John G. Neuhoff,et al.  Looming sounds are perceived as faster than receding sounds , 2016, Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications.

[12]  Altman Ja,et al.  Psychophysical Characteristics of the Auditory Image Movement Perception During Dichotic Stimulation , 1988 .

[13]  Tatsuya Hirahara,et al.  Perception of approaching and retreating sounds , 2015 .

[14]  Simon Carlile,et al.  The Perception of Auditory Motion , 2016, Trends in hearing.

[15]  J A Altman,et al.  Psychophysical characteristics of the auditory image movement perception during dichotic stimulation. , 1988, The International journal of neuroscience.

[16]  Charles Spence,et al.  Sensory determinants of the autonomous sensory meridian response (ASMR): understanding the triggers , 2017, PeerJ.

[17]  S. Whiteside,et al.  The Five Factor Model and impulsivity: using a structural model of personality to understand impulsivity , 2001 .

[18]  P. Loui,et al.  Thrills, chills, frissons, and skin orgasms: toward an integrative model of transcendent psychophysiological experiences in music , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[19]  G. Essick,et al.  Dependence of subjective traverse length on velocity of moving tactile stimuli. , 1986, Somatosensory research.

[20]  Erich Seifritz,et al.  Influence of anxiety, depression and looming cognitive style on auditory looming perception. , 2014, Journal of anxiety disorders.

[21]  Maciej Kłaczyński,et al.  Nuisance assessment of different annoying sounds based on psychoacoustic metrics and electroencephalography , 2016 .

[22]  Hugo Fastl,et al.  Psychoacoustics: Facts and Models , 1990 .

[23]  Eero P. Simoncelli,et al.  Summary statistics in auditory perception , 2013, Nature Neuroscience.

[24]  Nick J. Davis,et al.  Autonomous Sensory Meridian Response (ASMR): a flow-like mental state , 2015, PeerJ.

[25]  Erich Seifritz,et al.  Looming sounds as warning signals: the function of motion cues. , 2009, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[26]  福田 博一 State-Trait Anxiety Inventoryによるペインクリニック外来患者の不安の評価 , 1994 .

[27]  William J. Murphy,et al.  Revisiting the NIOSH Criteria for a Recommended Standard: Occupational Noise Exposure , 2002 .

[28]  Tomohisa Asai,et al.  Auditory multistability and neurotransmitter concentrations in the human brain , 2017, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.