"Shhh! We're Tryin' to Concentrate": Attention and Environmental Distracters in Novel Word Learning

The authors' purpose in this study was to evaluate the role of attention, as a central dimension of temperament, in children's real-time acquisition of novel vocabulary. Environmental distractions were administered to 47 22-month-old children as they acquired novel vocabulary in a fast-mapping task. Two distraction conditions impeded novel word acquisition, but only 1 impeded attention allocation. Attention allocation was correlated with novel word acquisition under conditions of distraction, but not in their absence. Results suggest that attention allocation is especially important for word learning under conditions of distraction. Given that in their day-to-day lives children often encounter new words amid a host of environmental distractions, children with constitutionally fewer attentional resources, such as temperamentally difficult children, may be at a vocabulary-learning disadvantage.

[1]  Rebecca J. Brand,et al.  Breaking the language barrier: an emergentist coalition model for the origins of word learning. , 2000, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[2]  Michael Morales,et al.  Gaze following, temperament, and language development in 6-month-olds: A replication and extension , 2000 .

[3]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Variability in early communicative development. , 1994, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[4]  K. Nelson,et al.  Temperament and language: Relations from toddlerhood to middle childhood. , 1992 .

[5]  C. Shore,et al.  Temperamental predictors of linguistic style during multiword acquisition , 1997 .

[6]  Lorraine E. Maxwell,et al.  Parental language and verbal responsiveness to children in crowded homes. , 1999, Developmental psychology.

[7]  Barbara K. Keogh,et al.  Temperament , 1982 .

[8]  Jan Karrass,et al.  Processes in language acquisition: the roles of gender, attention, and maternal encouragement of attention over time , 2002, Journal of Child Language.

[9]  Lisa M Oakes,et al.  Developmental changes in endogenous control of attention: the role of target familiarity on infants' distraction latency. , 2002, Child development.

[10]  A. Matheny,et al.  Bringing order out of chaos: Psychometric characteristics of the confusion, hubbub, and order scale , 1995 .

[11]  Martha Ann Bell,et al.  Working memory and inhibitory control in early childhood: Contributions from physiology, temperament, and language. , 2004, Developmental psychobiology.

[12]  H. Ruff,et al.  Development of attention and distractibility in the first 4 years of life. , 2003, Developmental psychology.

[13]  Lois Bloom,et al.  Language and Interaction. (Book Reviews: The Transition from Infancy to Language. Acquiring the Power of Expression.) , 1995 .

[14]  D. Carson,et al.  Comparisons of children with delayed and normal language at 24 months of age on measures of behavioral difficulties, social and cognitive development , 1998 .

[15]  W. Dixon,et al.  Links between Early Temperament and Language Acquisition. , 2000 .

[16]  Linda B. Smith,et al.  Whose DAM account? Attentional learning explains Booth and Waxman , 2003, Cognition.

[17]  Risto Näätänen,et al.  Effects of auditory distraction on electrophysiological brain activity and performance in children aged 8-13 years. , 2004, Psychophysiology.

[18]  Competition for Infants' Attention: The Interactive Influence of Attentional State and Stimulus Characteristics. , 2000, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[19]  J. Turnure,et al.  Distractibility and Concentration of Attention in Children's Development. , 1984 .

[20]  J. Richards,et al.  Extended visual fixation and distractibility in children from six to twenty-four months of age. , 2001, Child development.

[21]  J. Turnure Children's reactions to distractors in a learning situation. , 1970 .

[22]  M. Posner,et al.  Distress and attention interactions in early infancy. , 1997 .

[23]  C. Kopp,et al.  Compliance and Comprehension in Very Young Toddlers , 1990 .

[24]  Z. Šidák Rectangular Confidence Regions for the Means of Multivariate Normal Distributions , 1967 .

[25]  C. Mervis,et al.  Acquisition of the novel name--nameless category (N3C) principle. , 1994, Child development.

[26]  L. Oakes,et al.  The Influence of Attentional State and Stimulus Characteristics on Infant Distractibility , 1999 .

[27]  M. Rothbart,et al.  Attention in Early Development: Themes and Variations , 1996 .

[28]  M. Rothbart,et al.  Child Temperament and Parenting , 2002 .

[29]  J. Richards,et al.  Heart rate and behavioral measures of attention in six-, nine-, and twelve-month-old infants during object exploration. , 1997, Child development.

[30]  M. Rothbart,et al.  Studying infant temperament via the Revised Infant Behavior Questionnaire , 2003 .

[31]  I. Sigel,et al.  HANDBOOK OF CHILD PSYCHOLOGY , 2006 .

[32]  Marc H. Bornstein,et al.  Social, emotional, and personality development , 1988 .

[33]  M. Rothbart,et al.  Investigations of temperament at three to seven years: the Children's Behavior Questionnaire. , 2001, Child development.

[34]  L. Hakamies‐Blomqvist,et al.  Cognitive Development, Temperament and Behavior at 2 Years as Indicative of Language Development at 4 Years in Pre-Term Infants , 2001, Child psychiatry and human development.

[35]  L. Schmidt,et al.  Expressive and receptive language skills of temperamentally shy preschoolers , 2004 .

[36]  R. Yerkes,et al.  The relation of strength of stimulus to rapidity of habit‐formation , 1908 .

[37]  Wallace E Dixon,et al.  Temperament, distraction, and learning in toddlerhood. , 2006, Infant behavior & development.

[38]  Mary M. Humphrey Children's Avoidance of Environmental, Simple Task Internal, and Complex Task Internal Distractors. , 1982 .

[39]  M. Bornstein,et al.  Habituation and maternal encouragement of attention in infancy as predictors of toddler language, play, and representational competence. , 1989, Child development.