Models of scientific progress and the role of theory in taxonomy development: a case study of the DSM.

The proliferation of categories in recent editions of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (e.g., 4th ed.; DSM; American Psychiatric Association, 1994) is discussed as an indication that the underlying classification scheme is inadequate and unlikely to produce the scientific progress originally envisioned. In any nosological system, it eventually becomes necessary to reduce the number of categories by an organizing theory that describes the fundamental principles underlying the taxonomy. The DSM has put itself in an awkward position by claiming to be atheoretical. Although taking such a tack had historical advantages to promote the acceptability of the 3rd edition of the DSM, it now limits the progression of science. It is argued that the DSM should not be used as the basis for guiding scientific research programs because it emphasizes primarily behavioral topography rather than providing an explicit theory that would allow for an evaluation of scientific progress. Theoretically driven taxonomies should be allowed to compete on the basis of how successful they are at achieving their specified goals that might include illuminating etiology, course, and response to treatment. Such systems are not likely to attend primarily to behavioral topography alone and would probably organize behavior differently than the current categorical syndromes seen in the 4th edition of the DSM.

[1]  S D Imber,et al.  National Institute of Mental Health Treatment of Depression Collaborative Research Program. General effectiveness of treatments. , 1989, Archives of general psychiatry.

[2]  Dan J Stein,et al.  Cognitive Therapy of Substance Abuse , 1993, Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy.

[3]  John C. Shershow Schizophrenia : science and practice , 1978 .

[4]  P. Wiener,et al.  The Encyclopedia of Philosophy. , 1968 .

[5]  Samuel B. Guze,et al.  Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th ed. (DSM-IV) , 1995 .

[6]  R. Michels,et al.  Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 3rd ed , 1981 .

[7]  Stephen Jay Gould,et al.  Ever Since Darwin: Reflections in Natural History , 1977 .

[8]  M Agathon,et al.  [Behavior therapy]. , 1978, Psychotherapy and psychosomatics.

[9]  A. Beck Love is never enough : how couples can overcome misunderstandings, resolve conflicts, and solve relationship problems through cognitive therapy , 1989 .

[10]  D. Farner The growth of biological thought. Diversity, evolution, and inheritance , 1983 .

[11]  R. Kessler,et al.  Lifetime and 12-month prevalence of DSM-III-R psychiatric disorders in the United States. Results from the National Comorbidity Survey. , 1994, Archives of general psychiatry.

[12]  L. Laudan Science as Problem-Solving. (Book Reviews: Progress and Its Problems. Toward a Theory of Scientific Growth) , 1977 .

[13]  S. Kirk The Selling of DSM: The Rhetoric of Science in Psychiatry , 1992 .

[14]  Ronald N. Giere,et al.  Progress and its problems: Toward a theory of scientific growth , 1978 .

[15]  A. Fruzzetti Causes and consequences: individual distress in the context of couple interactions. , 1996, Journal of consulting and clinical psychology.

[16]  G. Klerman,et al.  Contemporary directions in psychopathology : toward the DSM-IV , 1986 .

[17]  J. Dietch Anxiety Disorders and Phobias: A Cognitive Perspective , 1986 .

[18]  M. Zimmerman Is DSM-IV needed at all? , 1990, Archives of general psychiatry.

[19]  R. Miner,et al.  The empiricist and his new clothes: DSM-III in perspective. , 1986, The American journal of psychiatry.

[20]  K. Popper,et al.  Conjectures and refutations;: The growth of scientific knowledge , 1972 .

[21]  Gary James Jason,et al.  The Logic of Scientific Discovery , 1988 .

[22]  T. Broadbent,et al.  Criticism and the Growth of Knowledge , 1972 .

[23]  E. Mayr The Growth of Biological Thought: Diversity, Evolution, and Inheritance , 1983 .

[24]  Dc Washington Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th Ed. , 1994 .

[25]  K. Popper,et al.  Conjectures and refutations;: The growth of scientific knowledge , 1972 .

[26]  Michael A. Schwartz,et al.  Philosophical Perspectives On Psychiatric Diagnostic Classification , 1994 .

[27]  V. Follette,et al.  A Behavior-Analytic View of Psychological Health , 1993, The Behavior analyst.

[28]  I. Lakatos Falsification and the Methodology of Scientific Research Programmes , 1976 .

[29]  R. Spitzer An outsider-insider's views about revising the DSMs. , 1991, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[30]  P. Meehl Bootstraps taxometrics. Solving the classification problem in psychopathology. , 1995, The American psychologist.

[31]  J. Sayers Against Method , 2016 .

[32]  W. Gove Deviance and mental illness , 1982 .

[33]  K. Popper,et al.  The Logic of Scientific Discovery , 1960 .

[34]  M. Resnik,et al.  Aspects of Scientific Explanation. , 1966 .

[35]  E. Mayr Toward a new philosophy of biology: observations of an evolutionist , 1988 .

[36]  D. Klein,et al.  Critical Issues in Psychiatric Diagnosis , 1978 .

[37]  W. O’Donohue,et al.  The (even) bolder model. The clinical psychologist as metaphysician-scientist-practitioner. , 1989, The American psychologist.

[38]  M. Drake Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (3rd ed. rev.) , 1988 .

[39]  J. Wakefield,et al.  Disorder as harmful dysfunction: a conceptual critique of DSM-III-R's definition of mental disorder. , 1992, Psychological review.

[40]  Norman MacBeth,et al.  Ever Since Darwin: Reflections on Natural History , 1978 .

[41]  M. Kovács Cognitive therapy in depression. , 1980, The Journal of the American Academy of Psychoanalysis.

[42]  R. Kendell Relationship between the DSM-IV and the ICD-10. , 1991, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[43]  W. Follette Correcting methodological weaknesses in the knowledge base used to derive practice standards. , 1995 .

[44]  H. Pincus,et al.  DSM-IV Guidebook , 1995 .

[45]  I. Elkin,et al.  NIMH Treatment of Depression Collaborative Research Program. Background and research plan. , 1985, Archives of general psychiatry.

[46]  S. Hayes Scientific standards of psychological practice : issues and recommendations , 1995 .

[47]  B. Skinner,et al.  The operational analysis of psychological terms , 1945, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[48]  Ian Tattersall,et al.  How We Know What We Think We Know About Human Evolution , 1995 .

[49]  Christopher Boorse,et al.  Health as a Theoretical Concept , 1977, Philosophy of Science.

[50]  C. Schmitt,et al.  Explanation in Science , 1973, Nature.

[51]  I. Lakatos,et al.  Criticism and the Growth of Knowledge: Falsification and the Methodology of Scientific Research Programmes , 1970 .

[52]  Roger K. Blashfield,et al.  The Classification of Psychopathology: Neo-Kraepelinian and Quantitative Approaches , 1984 .

[53]  J. Persons The advantages of studying psychological phenomena rather than psychiatric diagnoses. , 1986, The American psychologist.

[54]  M. Eagle Book Review:The Foundations of Psychoanalysis: A Philosophical Critique Adolf Grunbaum , 1986 .