PLAViMoP: How to standardize and simplify the use of point-light displays
暂无分享,去创建一个
Patrick Lacouture | Jean Pylouster | Yannick Blandin | Arnaud Decatoire | Sophie-Anne Beauprez | Christel Bidet-Ildei | Jean Pylouster | P. Lacouture | Christel Bidet-Ildei | Y. Blandin | Sophie-Anne Beauprez | A. Decatoire
[1] Christian Graff,et al. Four-Day-Old Human Neonates Look Longer at Non-Biological Motions of a Single Point-of-Light , 2007, PloS one.
[2] Marina Pavlova,et al. Perception of Elliptic Biological Motion , 2006, Perception.
[3] L. Proteau,et al. Cognitive Processes Underlying Observational Learning of Motor Skills , 1999 .
[4] T. Beardsworth,et al. The ability to recognize oneself from a video recording of one’s movements without seeing one’s body , 1981 .
[5] Randolph Blake,et al. Impaired visual recognition of biological motion in schizophrenia , 2005, Schizophrenia Research.
[6] J. Orliaguet,et al. Influence of Motor Disorders on the Visual Perception of Human Movements in a Case of Peripheral Dysgraphia , 2004, Neurocase.
[7] A. Mark Williams,et al. Looking to Learn: The Effects of Visual Guidance on Observational Learning of the Golf Swing , 2016, PloS one.
[8] J A Beintema,et al. Perception of biological motion without local image motion , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[9] Stefanie Hoehl,et al. The perception of biological motion by infants: An event-related potential study , 2006, Neuroscience Letters.
[10] A. Badets,et al. Painful semantic context modulates the relationship between action words and biological movement perception , 2017 .
[11] Hyun-seung Song,et al. The effect of action observation training on knee joint function and gait ability in total knee replacement patients , 2014, Journal of exercise rehabilitation.
[12] J. Cutting,et al. Recognizing the sex of a walker from a dynamic point-light display , 1977 .
[13] S. Small,et al. Action observation has a positive impact on rehabilitation of motor deficits after stroke , 2007, NeuroImage.
[14] T. J. Clarke,et al. The Perception of Emotion from Body Movement in Point-Light Displays of Interpersonal Dialogue , 2005, Perception.
[15] Eric Hiris,et al. Temporal properties in masking biological motion , 2005, Perception & psychophysics.
[16] M. Shiffrar,et al. The visual analysis of emotional actions , 2006, Social neuroscience.
[17] G. Gredebäck,et al. Humans Anticipate the Goal of other People’s Point-Light Actions , 2012, Front. Psychology.
[18] M. Shiffrar,et al. Recognizing people from their movement. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[19] D. Maurer,et al. The Development of Sensitivity to Biological Motion in Noise , 2006, Perception.
[20] Quoc C. Vuong,et al. First impressions: Gait cues drive reliable trait judgements , 2012, Cognition.
[21] D R Proffitt,et al. The development of infant sensitivity to biomechanical motions. , 1985, Child development.
[22] Christel Bidet-Ildei,et al. Influence of biological kinematics on abstract concept processing , 2015, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[23] Nikolaus F. Troje,et al. Structural encoding and recognition of biological motion: evidence from event-related potentials and source analysis , 2005, Behavioural Brain Research.
[24] Peter Neri,et al. Temporal dynamics of figure-ground segregation in human vision. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.
[25] Pär Nyström,et al. Human Infants Detect Other People's Interactions Based on Complex Patterns of Kinematic Information , 2014, PloS one.
[26] J. Cutting. Generation of Synthetic Male and Female Walkers through Manipulation of a Biomechanical Invariant , 1978, Perception.
[27] Daniel Jokisch,et al. Biological motion as a cue for the perception of size. , 2003, Journal of vision.
[28] Dorita H. F. Chang,et al. Characterizing global and local mechanisms in biological motion perception. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[29] M. Giese,et al. Learning to discriminate complex movements: biological versus artificial trajectories. , 2006, Journal of vision.
[30] L. Craighero,et al. Improving language without words: First evidence from aphasia , 2010, Neuropsychologia.
[31] Neil G. Muggleton,et al. Effects of TMS over Premotor and Superior Temporal Cortices on Biological Motion Perception , 2012, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[32] M. Pavlova,et al. Orientation specificity in biological motion perception , 2000, Perception & psychophysics.
[33] T. Pozzo,et al. Kinematic features of movement tunes perception and action coupling , 2006, Behavioural Brain Research.
[34] Eric Hiris,et al. What learning to see arbitrary motion tells us about biological motion perception. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[35] S. Sumi. Upside-down Presentation of the Johansson Moving Light-Spot Pattern , 1984, Perception.
[36] R. Blake,et al. Perception of human motion. , 2007, Annual review of psychology.
[37] Á. Pascual-Leone,et al. Repetitive TMS over posterior STS disrupts perception of biological motion , 2005, Vision Research.
[38] Marina Pavlova,et al. Prior Knowledge about Display Inversion in Biological Motion Perception , 2003, Perception.
[39] K. Verfaillie,et al. Bistability and Biasing Effects in the Perception of Ambiguous Point-Light Walkers , 2004, Perception.
[40] K. Nakayama,et al. Adaptation aftereffects in the perception of gender from biological motion. , 2006, Journal of vision.
[41] Kazuo Hiraki,et al. Active processing of biological motion perception: an ERP study. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[42] Emily D Grossman,et al. Temporal "Bubbles" reveal key features for point-light biological motion perception. , 2008, Journal of vision.
[43] R. Blake,et al. Brain Areas Involved in Perception of Biological Motion , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[44] Arnaud Barré,et al. Biomechanical ToolKit: Open-source framework to visualize and process biomechanical data , 2014, Comput. Methods Programs Biomed..
[45] D L Weeks,et al. The interaction of observational learning with overt practice: effects on motor skill learning. , 2000, Acta psychologica.
[46] Dorita H. F. Chang,et al. Perception of animacy and direction from local biological motion signals. , 2008, Journal of vision.
[47] B. Bertenthal,et al. Does Perception of Biological Motion Rely on Specific Brain Regions? , 2001, NeuroImage.
[48] J. Pineda,et al. Recognition of point-light biological motion: Mu rhythms and mirror neuron activity , 2007, Behavioural Brain Research.
[49] N. Troje,et al. The Inversion Effect in Biological Motion Perception: Evidence for a “Life Detector”? , 2006, Current Biology.
[50] S. Lea,et al. Perception of Emotion from Dynamic Point-Light Displays Represented in Dance , 1996, Perception.
[51] D. Wolpert,et al. Principles of sensorimotor learning , 2011, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[52] N. Hodges,et al. End-point focus manipulations to determine what information is used during observational learning. , 2007, Acta psychologica.
[53] Reza Langari,et al. Experimental observations on the human arm motion planning under an elbow joint constraint , 2012, 2012 Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society.
[54] K. Verfaillie. Perceiving Human Locomotion: Priming Effects in Direction Discrimination , 2000, Brain and Cognition.
[55] J. Mazziotta,et al. Grasping the Intentions of Others with One's Own Mirror Neuron System , 2005, PLoS biology.
[56] Bennett I. Bertenthal,et al. Global Processing of Biological Motions , 1994 .
[57] F. Binkofski,et al. Action observation as a tool for neurorehabilitation to moderate motor deficits and aphasia following stroke , 2012, Neural regeneration research.
[58] G. Orban,et al. Specificity of regions processing biological motion , 2005, The European journal of neuroscience.
[59] Lara Bardi,et al. The first time ever I saw your feet: inversion effect in newborns' sensitivity to biological motion. , 2014, Developmental psychology.
[60] Lara Bardi,et al. Biological motion preference in humans at birth: role of dynamic and configural properties. , 2011, Developmental science.
[61] Yann Coello,et al. Reading action word affects the visual perception of biological motion. , 2011, Acta psychologica.
[62] Randolph Blake,et al. Eccentric perception of biological motion is unscalably poor , 2005, Vision Research.
[63] M. Sereno,et al. Point-Light Biological Motion Perception Activates Human Premotor Cortex , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[64] Marina Pavlova,et al. Neuromagnetic Response to Body Motion and Brain Connectivity , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[65] David Whitney,et al. The psychophysics of visual motion and global form processing in autism. , 2010, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[66] R. Blake,et al. Brain Areas Active during Visual Perception of Biological Motion , 2002, Neuron.
[67] Yann Coello,et al. Anticipating the terminal position of an observed action: Effect of kinematic, structural, and identity information , 2011 .
[68] A. Klin,et al. Two-year-olds with autism orient to nonsocial contingencies rather than biological motion , 2009, Nature.
[69] Naomi B. Pitskel,et al. Neural signatures of autism , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[70] N. Troje,et al. Person identification from biological motion: Effects of structural and kinematic cues , 2005, Perception & psychophysics.
[71] J. Parkinson,et al. Movement kinematics affect action prediction: comparing human to non-human point-light actions , 2012, Psychological Research.
[72] Peter Neri,et al. Action Perception Is Intact in Autism Spectrum Disorder , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[73] P. Sinha,et al. Functional neuroanatomy of biological motion perception in humans , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[74] Christel Bidet-Ildei,et al. The Kinematics, Not the Orientation, of an Action Influences Language Processing , 2018, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[75] Christel Bidet-Ildei,et al. Sex Differences in the Neuromagnetic Cortical Response to Biological Motion. , 2015, Cerebral cortex.
[76] W. Dittrich. Action Categories and the Perception of Biological Motion , 1993, Perception.
[77] S. Swinnen,et al. Recognizing Biological Motion and Emotions from Point-Light Displays in Autism Spectrum Disorders , 2012, PloS one.
[78] Luc Proteau,et al. Learning through observation: a combination of expert and novice models favors learning , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.
[79] N. Troje. Decomposing biological motion: a framework for analysis and synthesis of human gait patterns. , 2002, Journal of vision.
[80] G. Johansson. Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis , 1973 .
[81] A. Chauvin,et al. Observing or producing a motor action improves later perception of biological motion: evidence for a gender effect. , 2010, Acta psychologica.
[82] Philippe Lefèvre,et al. Biological motion drives perception and action. , 2010, Journal of vision.
[83] Roel M. Willems,et al. Neural evidence for the interplay between language, gesture, and action: A review , 2007, Brain and Language.
[84] Luc Proteau,et al. Specificity of Learning in a Video-Aiming Task: Modifying the Salience of Dynamic Visual Cues , 2005, Journal of motor behavior.
[85] A. Young,et al. Emotion Perception from Dynamic and Static Body Expressions in Point-Light and Full-Light Displays , 2004, Perception.
[86] Gavin Breslin,et al. Effect of Information Load and Time on Observational Learning , 2009, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.
[87] Robert R. Horn,et al. The efficacy of demonstrations in teaching children an unfamiliar movement skill: The effects of object-orientated actions and point-light demonstrations , 2007, Journal of sports sciences.
[88] K. Hiraki,et al. An event-related potentials study of biological motion perception in human infants. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[89] L. Avanzino,et al. Action Observation and Motor Imagery: Innovative Cognitive Tools in the Rehabilitation of Parkinson's Disease , 2015, Parkinson's disease.
[90] Luc Proteau,et al. Mixed observation favors motor learning through better estimation of the model’s performance , 2014, Experimental Brain Research.
[91] Randolph Blake,et al. A new technique for generating disordered point-light animations for the study of biological motion perception. , 2015, Journal of vision.
[92] A. Badets,et al. Sentence plausibility influences the link between action words and the perception of biological human movements , 2017, Psychological research.
[93] Alireza Gharabaghi,et al. Cerebellar engagement in an action observation network. , 2010, Cerebral cortex.
[94] N. Birbaumer,et al. Recognition of Point-Light Biological Motion Displays by Young Children , 2001, Perception.
[95] Y. Petrov,et al. Depth perception in the framework of General Object Constancy. , 2013, Journal of vision.
[96] Jim W Kay,et al. Gender recognition from point-light walkers. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[97] Hongjing Lu,et al. A biological motion toolbox for reading, displaying, and manipulating motion capture data in research settings. , 2013, Journal of vision.
[98] T. Shipley. The Effect of Object and Event Orientation on Perception of Biological Motion , 2003, Psychological science.
[99] P. Bennett,et al. Effects of aging on biological motion discrimination , 2010, Vision Research.
[100] S. Runeson,et al. Visual perception of lifted weight. , 1981, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[101] T. Poggio,et al. Cognitive neuroscience: Neural mechanisms for the recognition of biological movements , 2003, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[102] M. Pavlova. Biological motion processing as a hallmark of social cognition. , 2012, Cerebral cortex.
[103] N. Troje,et al. Healthy older observers cannot use biological-motion point-light information efficiently within 4 m of themselves , 2012, i-Perception.
[104] G. Buccino,et al. Action observation versus motor imagery in learning a complex motor task: A short review of literature and a kinematics study , 2013, Neuroscience Letters.
[105] Ronald A. Rensink,et al. Active versus passive processing of biological motion , 2002, Perception.
[106] Nikolaus F. Troje,et al. Perception of biological motion in autism spectrum disorders , 2008, Neuropsychologia.
[107] J Decety,et al. Is perceptual anticipation a motor simulation? A PET study , 2001, Neuroreport.
[108] Kevin A. Pelphrey,et al. Sex differences in the development of brain mechanisms for processing biological motion , 2013, NeuroImage.
[109] Y. Coello,et al. Rôle des représentations motrices dans la perception visuelle des mouvements humains , 2011, L’Année psychologique.
[110] Edouard Gentaz,et al. Preference for point-light human biological motion in newborns: contribution of translational displacement. , 2014, Developmental psychology.
[111] M. Shiffrar,et al. The visual perception of human locomotion. , 1998, Cognitive neuropsychology.
[112] S. Vogt,et al. From visuo-motor interactions to imitation learning: Behavioural and brain imaging studies , 2007, Journal of sports sciences.
[113] R. Blake,et al. Brain activity evoked by inverted and imagined biological motion , 2001, Vision Research.
[114] Rolf A. Zwaan,et al. Embodied Language: A Review of the Role of the Motor System in Language Comprehension , 2008, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[115] M. Giese,et al. Effects of aging on identifying emotions conveyed by point-light walkers. , 2016, Psychology and aging.
[116] Jong-Min Lee,et al. BOLD response during visual perception of biological motion in obsessive-compulsive disorder , 2009, European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience.
[117] Reza Langari,et al. Experimental Observations on Human Reaching Motion Planning With and Without a Reduced Mobility , 2015 .
[118] Jennifer M. D. Yoon,et al. Biological motion displays elicit social behavior in 12-month-olds. , 2009, Child development.
[119] S. Dehaene. Varieties of numerical abilities , 1992, Cognition.
[120] B. Bertenthal,et al. Infant sensitivity to figural coherence in biomechanical motions. , 1984, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[121] Karl Verfaillie,et al. Perceiving the Direction of Articulatory Motion in Point-Light Actions , 2014, PloS one.
[122] Thierry Pozzo,et al. Electrophysiological correlates of biological motion permanence in humans , 2013, Behavioural Brain Research.
[123] Friedemann Pulvermüller,et al. Brain mechanisms linking language and action , 2005, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[124] M. Shiffrar,et al. Subconfigurations of the human form in the perception of biological motion displays. , 1999, Acta psychologica.
[125] J E Cutting,et al. Masking the motions of human gait , 1988, Perception & psychophysics.
[126] Eric Hiris,et al. Detection of biological and nonbiological motion. , 2007, Journal of vision.
[127] Christel Bidet-Ildei,et al. Are judgments for action verbs and point-light human actions equivalent? , 2014, Cognitive Processing.
[128] K. Verfaillie,et al. Viewpoint-dependent Priming Effects in the Perception of Human Actions and Body Postures , 1999 .
[129] T. Shipley,et al. Influence of the perception of biological or non-biological motion on movement execution , 2007, Journal of sports sciences.
[130] F. Simion,et al. A predisposition for biological motion in the newborn baby , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[131] Emily D. Grossman,et al. Necessary but not sufficient: Motion perception is required for perceiving biological motion , 2008, Vision Research.
[132] G. Buccino,et al. Action observation treatment improves recovery of postsurgical orthopedic patients: evidence for a top-down effect? , 2010, Archives of physical medicine and rehabilitation.
[133] H. Bülthoff,et al. Attentional Networks and Biological Motion , 2010 .
[134] D R Proffitt,et al. Perception of biomechanical motions by infants: implementation of various processing constraints. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[135] Steven M. Thurman,et al. Perception of Social Interactions for Spatially Scrambled Biological Motion , 2014, PloS one.
[136] Kristian M O'Connor,et al. Assessment of a greater trochanter-based method of locating the hip joint center. , 2010, Journal of biomechanics.
[137] Takeo Kanade,et al. Ambiguities in Visual Tracking of Articulated Objects Using Two- and Three-Dimensional Models , 2003, Int. J. Robotics Res..
[138] Marina Pavlova,et al. Perception and production of biological movement in patients with early periventricular brain lesions. , 2003, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[139] Alan C. Evans,et al. Specific Involvement of Human Parietal Systems and the Amygdala in the Perception of Biological Motion , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[140] Alison J. Wiggett,et al. Patterns of fMRI Activity Dissociate Overlapping Functional Brain Areas that Respond to Biological Motion , 2006, Neuron.
[141] Christel Bidet-Ildei,et al. Visual preference for isochronic movement does not necessarily emerge from movement kinematics: A challenge for the motor simulation theory , 2008, Neuroscience Letters.
[142] Robert R Horn,et al. Learning from demonstrations: the role of visual search during observational learning from video and point-light models , 2002, Journal of sports sciences.
[143] Perceiving a Biological Human Movement Facilitates Action Verb Processing , 2019 .