Talking nets: a multiagent connectionist approach to communication and trust between individuals.

A multiagent connectionist model is proposed that consists of a collection of individual recurrent networks that communicate with each other and, as such, is a network of networks. The individual recurrent networks simulate the process of information uptake, integration, and memorization within individual agents, and the communication of beliefs and opinions between agents is propagated along connections between the individual networks. A crucial aspect in belief updating based on information from other agents is the trust in the information provided. In the model, trust is determined by the consistency with the receiving agents' existing beliefs and results in changes of the connections between individual networks, called trust weights. These weights lead to a selective propagation and thus to the filtering out of less reliable information, and they implement H. P. Grice's (1975) maxims of quality and quantity in communication. The unique contribution of communicative mechanisms beyond intrapersonal processing of individual networks was explored in simulations of key phenomena involving persuasive communication and polarization, lexical acquisition, spreading of stereotypes and rumors, and a lack of sharing unique information in group decisions.

[1]  Antonio R. Damasio,et al.  The interaction of affect and cognition: A neurobiological perspective. , 2000 .

[2]  Olivier Klein,et al.  Hidden profiles and the consensualization of social stereotypes: how information distribution affects stereotype content and sharedness , 2003 .

[3]  J Richard Eiser,et al.  Connectionist Simulation of Attitude Learning: Asymmetries in the Acquisition of Positive and Negative Evaluations , 2003, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[4]  Philipp Slusallek,et al.  Introduction to real-time ray tracing , 2005, SIGGRAPH Courses.

[5]  P. Gollwitzer Action phases and mind-sets , 1990 .

[6]  T. Shultz,et al.  Cognitive dissonance reduction as constraint satisfaction. , 1996, Psychological review.

[7]  Geoffrey Haddock Subjective ease of retrieval and attitude-relevant judgments. , 2000 .

[8]  Yoshihisa Kashima,et al.  A Distributed Memory Model of Averaging Phenomena in Person Impression Formation , 1994 .

[9]  William A. Cunningham,et al.  Neural components of social evaluation. , 2003, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[10]  E. Rosch,et al.  Categorization of Natural Objects , 1981 .

[11]  P. Thagard,et al.  FORMING IMPRESSIONS FROM STEREOTYPES, TRAITS, AND BEHAVIORS : A PARALLEL-CONSTRAINT-SATISFACTION THEORY , 1996 .

[12]  Edwin Hutchins,et al.  The social organization of distributed cognition , 1991, Perspectives on socially shared cognition.

[13]  Francis Heylighen,et al.  Collective Intelligence and its Implementation on the Web: Algorithms to Develop a Collective Mental Map , 1999, Comput. Math. Organ. Theory.

[14]  Janet B. Ruscher,et al.  Multiple communicators with unique target information transmit less stereotypical impressions , 1998 .

[15]  S. Thorpe Localized versus distributed representations , 1998 .

[16]  I. Ajzen,et al.  Belief, Attitude, Intention, and Behavior: An Introduction to Theory and Research , 1977 .

[17]  Michael A. Olson,et al.  Implicit Attitude Formation Through Classical Conditioning , 2001, Psychological science.

[18]  S. Moscovici,et al.  Biased information search in group decision making. , 2000, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology.

[19]  E. Tory Higgins,et al.  Primacy and recency in communication and self-persuasion : how successive audiences and multiple encodings influence subsequent evaluative judgments , 1991 .

[20]  C. Stangor,et al.  Memory for expectancy-congruent and expectancy-incongruent information: A review of the social and social developmental literatures. , 1992 .

[21]  F. Heylighen What makes a meme successful? Selection criteria for cultural evolution , 1998 .

[22]  Ralph R. Miller,et al.  Assessment of the Rescorla-Wagner model. , 1995 .

[23]  Alan M. Lesgold,et al.  Coherence: Beyond constraint satisfaction , 1989, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[24]  Linda L. Putnam,et al.  Handbook of Organizational Communication: An Interdisciplinary Perspective , 1987 .

[25]  R. Sun Cognitive science meets multi-agent systems: A prolegomenon , 2001 .

[26]  N. Anderson Integration theory and attitude change. , 1971 .

[27]  Charles E. Miller,et al.  Communication Networks in Task-Performing Groups , 2000 .

[28]  Ebbe B. Ebbesen,et al.  Proportion of risky to conservative arguments in a group discussion and choice shift. , 1974 .

[29]  K. Mackenzie A Theory of Group Structures , 2016 .

[30]  Rick L. Riolo,et al.  Beyond Geography: Cooperation with Persistent Links in the Absence of Clustered Neighborhoods , 2002 .

[31]  Barbara A. Spellman,et al.  If Saddam is Hitler then who is George Bush? Analogical mapping between systems of social roles , 1992 .

[32]  L. Steels Evolving grounded communication for robots , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[33]  Leila T. Worth,et al.  Processing deficits and the mediation of positive affect in persuasion. , 1989, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[34]  R. Krauss,et al.  Concurrent feedback, confirmation, and the encoding of referents in verbal communication. , 1966, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[35]  H. Kelley Causal schemata and the attribution process , 1972 .

[36]  Tilmann Betsch,et al.  The value-account model of attitude formation , 2004 .

[37]  M. Bradley,et al.  Neuroanatomical correlates of pleasant and unpleasant emotion , 1997, Neuropsychologia.

[38]  S. Chaiken,et al.  When Parsimony Fails. .. , 1999 .

[39]  Alberto Leon-Garcia,et al.  Communication Networks , 2000 .

[40]  Diane M. Mackie,et al.  Systematic and nonsystematic processing of majority and minority persuasive communications. , 1987 .

[41]  Herbert H. Clark,et al.  Coordinating beliefs in conversation , 1992 .

[42]  Charles M. Judd,et al.  The Consequences of Communicating Social Stereotypes , 2000 .

[43]  Curtis P. Haugtvedt,et al.  Advertising Repetition and Variation Strategies: Implications for Understanding Attitude Strength , 1994 .

[44]  S. Read,et al.  Rapist or "Regular Guy": Explanatory Coherence in the Construction of Mental Models of Others , 1993 .

[45]  Dale J. Barr,et al.  Establishing conventional communication systems: Is common knowledge necessary? , 2004, Cogn. Sci..

[46]  Gün R. Semin,et al.  Language, interaction and social cognition , 1992 .

[47]  S. Chaiken,et al.  The generality of the automatic attitude activation effect. , 1992, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[48]  Eliot R. Smith,et al.  Dual-Process Models in Social and Cognitive Psychology: Conceptual Integration and Links to Underlying Memory Systems , 2000 .

[49]  E. Hutchins Cognition in the wild , 1995 .

[50]  L. Festinger A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance , 1957 .

[51]  D. Schacter,et al.  Do Amnesics Exhibit Cognitive Dissonance Reduction? The Role of Explicit Memory and Attention in Attitude Change , 2001, Psychological science.

[52]  G. Clore,et al.  Mood, misattribution, and judgments of well-being: Informative and directive functions of affective states. , 1983 .

[53]  Sabine A. Einwiller,et al.  Accuracy Motivation, Consensus Information, and the Law of Large Numbers: Effects on Attitude Judgment in the Absence of Argumentation , 1998 .

[54]  Robert M. French,et al.  Pseudo-recurrent Connectionist Networks: An Approach to the 'Sensitivity-Stability' Dilemma , 1997, Connect. Sci..

[55]  S. Chaiken,et al.  Dual-process theories in social psychology , 1999 .

[56]  F. Försterling,et al.  Models of covariation and attribution: How do they relate to the analogy of analysis of variance? , 1989 .

[57]  Gerhard Weiss,et al.  Multiagent systems: a modern approach to distributed artificial intelligence , 1999 .

[58]  Scott LeeTiernan,et al.  Personality as a Dynamical System: Emergence of Stability and Distinctiveness from Intra and Interpersonal Interactions , 2002 .

[59]  J. Kruschke,et al.  ALCOVE: an exemplar-based connectionist model of category learning. , 1992, Psychological review.

[60]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  The Effects of Involvement on Responses to Argument Quantity and Quality: Central and Peripheral Routes to Persuasion , 1984 .

[61]  I. Ajzen Residual Effects of Past on Later Behavior: Habituation and Reasoned Action Perspectives , 2002 .

[62]  Reid Hastie,et al.  A computer simulation of jury decision making. , 1980 .

[63]  S. Chaiken,et al.  Audience response as a heuristic cue in persuasion. , 1987, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[64]  William K. Estes,et al.  Classification and cognition , 1994 .

[65]  Bernard Ans,et al.  Neural networks with a self-refreshing memory: Knowledge transfer in sequential learning tasks without catastrophic forgetting , 2000, Connect. Sci..

[66]  J. Levine,et al.  Shared Cognition in-Organizations: The Management of Knowledge , 1999 .

[67]  E. Burnstein,et al.  Encoding under trust and distrust: the spontaneous activation of incongruent cognitions. , 2004, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[68]  Herbert Bless,et al.  Subjective Experience Versus Content of Information in the Construction of Attitude Judgments , 1996 .

[69]  Peter R. Monge,et al.  Emergent communication networks. , 1987 .

[70]  Michelene T. H. Chi,et al.  Assimilating evidence: The key to revision? , 1989, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[71]  Denise M. Driscoll,et al.  Cognitive organization of impressions: effects of incongruency in complex representations , 1989 .

[72]  F. Vallée-Tourangeau,et al.  Selective associations and causality judgments: Presence of a strong causal factor may reduce judgments of a weaker one. , 1993 .

[73]  R. D. Hansen,et al.  Discounting and augmenting facilitative and inhibitory forces: The winner takes almost all. , 1985 .

[74]  S. Quartz,et al.  Getting to Know You: Reputation and Trust in a Two-Person Economic Exchange , 2005, Science.

[75]  Brian Hazlehurst,et al.  How to invent a lexicon: the development of shared symbols in interaction , 2006 .

[76]  Jonathan M. Bowman,et al.  A social validation explanation for mutual enhancement. , 2004 .

[77]  Gerd Bohner,et al.  Beyond Conflict and Discrepancy: Cognitive Bias in Minority and Majority Influence , 1998 .

[78]  Frank Van Overwalle,et al.  Spontaneous Trait Inferences: Are They Linked to the Actor or to the Action? , 1999 .

[79]  I. Janis Victims Of Groupthink , 1972 .

[80]  H. Plessner,et al.  PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN Betsch et al. / IMPLICIT ATTITUDE FORMATION I Like It but I Don’t Know Why: A Value-Account Approach to Implicit Attitude Formation , 2022 .

[81]  C. Staats,et al.  Attitudes established by classical conditioning. , 1958, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[82]  Wander Jager,et al.  Fashions, habits and changing preferences: Simulation of psychological factors affecting market dynamics , 2001 .

[83]  I. Ajzen The theory of planned behavior , 1991 .

[84]  W. Estes,et al.  Base-rate effects in category learning: a comparison of parallel network and memory storage-retrieval models. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[85]  Koch Sigmund Ed,et al.  Psychology: A Study of A Science , 1962 .

[86]  Nigel Gilbert,et al.  Cities can be agents too: a model for the evolution of settlement systems , 2006 .

[87]  Laura R. Novick,et al.  A probabilistic contrast model of causal induction. , 1990 .

[88]  Michael A. Becker Social Psychology: Handbook of Basic Principles , 1998 .

[89]  P. Senge The fifth discipline : the art and practice of the learning organization/ Peter M. Senge , 1991 .

[90]  I. Ajzen,et al.  Prediction of goal directed behaviour: Attitudes, intentions and perceived behavioural control , 1986 .

[91]  G. Stasser,et al.  Pooling of Unshared Information in Group Decision Making: Biased Information Sampling During Discussion , 1985 .

[92]  Susan R. Fussell,et al.  Social psychological models of interpersonal communication , 1996 .

[93]  Garold Stasser,et al.  Computer simulation as a research tool: The DISCUSS model of group decision making , 1988 .

[94]  Stephen J. Read,et al.  An autoassociative model of causal reasoning and causal learning : Reply to Van Overwalle's (1998) critique of Read and Marcus-Newhall (1993) , 1999 .

[95]  G. Bohner,et al.  When expertise backfires: contrast and assimilation effects in persuasion. , 2002, The British journal of social psychology.

[96]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  Personal involvement as a determinant of argument based persuasion , 1981 .

[97]  Shelly Chaiken,et al.  The heuristic-systematic model in its broader context. , 1999 .

[98]  R. Krauss,et al.  Changes in reference phrases as a function of frequency of usage in social interaction: a preliminary study , 1964 .

[99]  M. J. Rosenberg,et al.  Attitude organization and change. An analysis of consistency among attitude components. , 1961 .

[100]  Frank Van Overwalle,et al.  Causal explanation as constraint satisfaction : A critique and a feedforward connectionist alternative , 1998 .

[101]  Keith J. Holyoak,et al.  A Coherence Model of Cognitive Consistency: Dynamics of Attitude Change During the Persian Gulf War , 1993 .

[102]  W. F. Prokasy,et al.  Classical conditioning II: Current research and theory. , 1972 .

[103]  R. Fazio,et al.  Attitude Accessibility and Motivation as Determinants of Biased Processing: A Test of the MODE Model , 1995 .

[104]  Jodie Woolcock,et al.  Group impressions as dynamic configurations: the tensor product model of group impression formation and change. , 2000, Psychological review.

[105]  S. Chaiken,et al.  Promoting systematic processing in low-motivation settings: effect of incongruent information on processing and judgment. , 1991, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[106]  R Ratcliff,et al.  Connectionist models of recognition memory: constraints imposed by learning and forgetting functions. , 1990, Psychological review.

[107]  N. Schwarz Feelings as information: Informational and motivational functions of affective states. , 1990 .

[108]  Nancy E. Avis,et al.  Base rates can affect individual predictions. , 1980 .

[109]  D. Shanks On Similarities between Causal Judgments in Experienced and Described Situations , 1991 .

[110]  Michaela Wänke,et al.  The effects of subjective ease of retrieval on attitudinal judgments : The moderating role of processing motivation , 2000 .

[111]  Geoffrey E. Hinton,et al.  Schemata and Sequential Thought Processes in PDP Models , 1986 .

[112]  G. Bower,et al.  From conditioning to category learning: an adaptive network model. , 1988 .

[113]  R. Fazio Multiple Processes by which Attitudes Guide Behavior: The Mode Model as an Integrative Framework , 1990 .

[114]  K. Kaplan On the ambivalence-indifference problem in attitude theory and measurement: A suggested modification of the semantic differential technique. , 1972 .

[115]  J. de Houwer,et al.  Associative learning of likes and dislikes: a review of 25 years of research on human evaluative conditioning. , 2001, Psychological bulletin.

[116]  D. LaBerge Attention, Awareness, and the Triangular Circuit , 1997, Consciousness and Cognition.

[117]  Yoshihisa Kashima,et al.  Maintaining Cultural Stereotypes in the Serial Reproduction of Narratives , 2000 .

[118]  David R. Shanks,et al.  CATEGORIZATION BY A CONNECTIONIST NETWORK , 1991 .

[119]  De Vries Book review: R.C. O'Reilly and Y. Munakata: Computational explorations in cognitive neuroscience: understanding the mind by stimulating the brain. Cambridge, Mass: The MIT Press. , 2002 .

[120]  G. Clore,et al.  Mood-Related Persuasion Depends on (Mis)Attributions , 1994 .

[121]  A. Kruglanski,et al.  Persuasion by a Single Route: A View From the Unimodel , 1999 .

[122]  Matthew D. Lieberman,et al.  The emergence of social cognitive neuroscience. , 2001, The American psychologist.

[123]  Ron Sun,et al.  Multi-agent reinforcement learning: weighting and partitioning , 1999, Neural Networks.

[124]  James L. McClelland,et al.  On learning the past-tenses of English verbs: implicit rules or parallel distributed processing , 1986 .

[125]  E. Higgins,et al.  Handbook of motivation and cognition : foundations of social behavior , 1991 .

[126]  Edmund T. Rolls,et al.  Introduction to Connectionist Modelling of Cognitive Processes , 1998 .

[127]  L. Steels,et al.  coordinating perceptually grounded categories through language: a case study for colour , 2005, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[128]  S. Chaiken,et al.  Heuristic processing can bias systematic processing: effects of source credibility, argument ambiguity, and task importance on attitude judgment. , 1994, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[129]  Wendy Wood,et al.  Access to attitude-relevant information in memory as a determinant of persuasion: The role of message attributes , 1985 .

[130]  D. Hilton,et al.  Processes of causal explanation and dispositional attribution. , 1995 .

[131]  Timothy D. Wilson,et al.  A model of dual attitudes. , 2000, Psychological review.

[132]  Frank Van Overwalle,et al.  Dispositional Attributions Require the Joint Application of the Methods of Difference and Agreement , 1997 .

[133]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Distributed memory and the representation of general and specific information. , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[134]  M. Diehl,et al.  Effects of Attitudinal Ambivalence on Information Processing and Attitude-Intention Consistency☆ , 1997 .

[135]  E. E. Jones,et al.  Decision freedom as a determinant of the role of incentive magnitude in attitude change. , 1967 .

[136]  E. E. Jones Attribution: Perceiving the Causes of Behavior , 1987 .

[137]  Michael A. Rodriguez,et al.  Societal-Scale Decision Making Using Social Networks , 2004 .

[138]  Luc Steels,et al.  The emergence and evolution of linguistic structure: from lexical to grammatical communication systems , 2005, Connect. Sci..

[139]  H. Kelley Attribution in social interaction. , 1987 .

[140]  Janet B. Ruscher,et al.  Prejudice and Stereotyping in Everyday Communication , 1998 .

[141]  L. Berkowitz,et al.  Label-mediated hostility generalation. , 1969, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[142]  David R. Roskos-Ewoldsen,et al.  The Influence of Accessibility of Source Likability on Persuasion , 2002 .

[143]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Explorations in parallel distributed processing: a handbook of models, programs, and exercises , 1988 .

[144]  J. Forgas Handbook of Affect and Social Cognition , 2001 .

[145]  Marco Dorigo,et al.  Swarm intelligence: from natural to artificial systems , 1999 .

[146]  S. Chaiken The heuristic model of persuasion. , 1987 .

[147]  Andrzej Nowak,et al.  Computer modeling of social processes , 1998 .

[148]  Yoshihisa Kashima,et al.  How are stereotypes maintained through communication? The influence of stereotype sharedness. , 2003, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[149]  Klaus Fiedler,et al.  The auto-verification of social hypotheses: Stereotyping and the power of sample size. , 1999 .

[150]  Joseph P. Forgas,et al.  The Message Within : The Role of Subjective Experience In Social Cognition And Behavior , 2000 .

[151]  H. H. Clark,et al.  Understanding by addressees and overhearers , 1989, Cognitive Psychology.

[152]  Garold Stasser,et al.  The uncertain role of unshared information in collective choice. , 1999 .

[153]  G. Chapman,et al.  Trial order affects cue interaction in contingency judgment. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[154]  Joshua M. Epstein,et al.  Growing Artificial Societies: Social Science from the Bottom Up , 1996 .

[155]  D. Isenberg Group polarization: A critical review and meta-analysis. , 1986 .

[156]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  Central and Peripheral Routes to Advertising Effectiveness: The Moderating Role of Involvement , 1983 .

[157]  Chester A. Insko,et al.  The Cognitive Mediation Hypothesis Revisited: An Empirical Response to Methodological and Theoretical Criticism , 1996 .

[158]  E. Wasserman,et al.  Cue Competition in Causality Judgments: The Role of Nonpresentation of Compound Stimulus Elements , 1994 .

[159]  David W. Schumann,et al.  Predicting the Effectiveness of Different Strategies of Advertising Variation: A Test of the Repetition-Variation Hypotheses , 1990 .

[160]  F. Van Overwalle,et al.  An Adaptive Connectionist Model of Cognitive Dissonance , 2007 .

[161]  Joel Cooper,et al.  Attitude polarization: Effects of group membership. , 1984 .

[162]  R. Riolo,et al.  Evolution of cooperation without reciprocity , 2001, Nature.

[163]  J. R. Larson,et al.  Leadership Style and the Discussion of Shared and Unshared Information in Decision-Making Groups , 1998 .

[164]  Duane T. Wegener,et al.  The elaboration likelihood model: Current status and controversies. , 1999 .

[165]  N. Anderson,et al.  Averaging model analysis of set-size effect in impression formation. , 1967, Journal of experimental psychology.

[166]  K. Fiedler Explaining and simulating judgment biases as an aggregation phenomenon in probabilistic, multiple-cue environments. , 1996 .

[167]  Alice H. Eagly,et al.  Heuristic and systematic information processing within and beyond the persuasion context. , 1989 .

[168]  Anthony G. Greenwald,et al.  Psychological foundations of attitudes , 1968 .

[169]  C A Kiesler,et al.  Positive and negative attitudinal affect established by classical conditioning. , 1970, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[170]  N. Schwarz,et al.  Mood and Persuasion: Affective States Influence the Processing of Persuasive Communications , 1991 .

[171]  Stephanie D. Teasley,et al.  Perspectives on socially shared cognition , 1991 .

[172]  Eliot R. Smith,et al.  Knowledge acquisition, accessibility, and use in person perception and stereotyping: simulation with a recurrent connectionist network. , 1998, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[173]  J. Desmond,et al.  Hemispheric asymmetry for emotional stimuli detected with fMRI , 1998, Neuroreport.

[174]  John T. Cacioppo,et al.  Affect and Attitudes: A Social Neuroscience Approach , 2000 .

[175]  Bernard Ans,et al.  Avoiding catastrophic forgetting by coupling two reverberating neural networks , 1997 .

[176]  S. Chaiken Heuristic versus systematic information processing and the use of source versus message cues in persuasion. , 1980 .

[177]  Eliot R. Smith,et al.  Subtyping versus bookkeeping in stereotype learning and change: connectionist simulations and empirical findings. , 2002, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[178]  C. McCann,et al.  Personal and contextual factors in communication: A review of the 'communication game.' , 1992 .

[179]  Siobhan Chapman Logic and Conversation , 2005 .

[180]  David R. Shanks,et al.  Distinguishing Associative and Probabilistic Contrast Theories of Human Contingency Judgment , 1996 .

[181]  B. Latané,et al.  From private attitude to public opinion: A dynamic theory of social impact. , 1990 .

[182]  Susan R. Fussell,et al.  Constructing shared communicative environments , 1991, Perspectives on socially shared cognition.

[183]  E. Higgins,et al.  Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles. , 1996 .

[184]  Russell H. Fazio,et al.  On the Value of Knowing One's Likes and Dislikes: Attitude Accessibility, Stress, and Health in College , 1997 .

[185]  J. Kruschke,et al.  A model of probabilistic category learning. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[186]  R. French,et al.  Catastrophic Forgetting in Connectionist Networks: Causes, Consequences and Solutions , 1994 .

[187]  Fredric M. Jablin Superior–subordinate communication: The state of the art. , 1979 .

[188]  M. J. Rosenberg Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Components of Attitudes , 1960 .

[189]  A. Pentland,et al.  Collective intelligence , 2006, IEEE Comput. Intell. Mag..

[190]  Richard E Petty,et al.  Contextual moderation of racial bias: the impact of social roles on controlled and automatically activated attitudes. , 2004, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[191]  Gerald Echterhoff,et al.  Audience-tuning effects on memory: the role of shared reality. , 2005, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[192]  G. Chapman,et al.  Cue interaction in human contingency judgment , 1990, Memory & cognition.

[193]  J. Cooper,et al.  A New Look at Dissonance Theory , 1984 .

[194]  M. Riketta,et al.  Manipulating self-esteem with subliminally presented words. , 2003 .

[195]  John T. Cacioppo,et al.  The Elaboration Likelihood Model of Persuasion , 1986, Advances in Experimental Social Psychology.

[196]  Russell H. Fazio,et al.  Attitude Accessibility as a Function of Repeated Attitudinal Expression , 1984 .

[197]  Axel Cleeremans,et al.  Implicit learning out of the lab: the case of orthographic regularities. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[198]  Diane M. Mackie,et al.  Cognitive Mediation of Positive Affect in Persuasion , 1987 .

[199]  S. Ratneshwar,et al.  The Effect of Cultural Orientation on Persuasion , 1997 .

[200]  G. Bohner,et al.  Negative Affect Can Increase or Decrease Message Scrutiny: The Affect Interpretation Hypothesis , 2001 .

[201]  F. Van Overwalle,et al.  A Connectionist Model of Attitude Formation and Change , 2005, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.

[202]  H. Leavitt Some effects of certain communication patterns on group performance. , 1951, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[203]  R. Rescorla,et al.  A theory of Pavlovian conditioning : Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement , 1972 .

[204]  Michaela Wänke,et al.  There Are Many Reasons to Drive a BMW: Does Imagined Ease of Argument Generation Influence Attitudes? , 1997 .

[205]  R. Petty,et al.  The gradual threshold model of ambivalence: relating the positive and negative bases of attitudes to subjective ambivalence. , 1996, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[206]  N. Anderson Foundations of information integration theory , 1981 .

[207]  Michael McCloskey,et al.  Catastrophic Interference in Connectionist Networks: The Sequential Learning Problem , 1989 .

[208]  A. Greenwald 6 – Cognitive Learning, Cognitive Response to Persuasion, and Attitude Change1 , 1968 .

[209]  I. Ajzen Attitudes, Personality and Behavior , 1988 .

[210]  R. O’Reilly,et al.  Conjunctive representations in learning and memory: principles of cortical and hippocampal function. , 2001, Psychological review.

[211]  S. Chaiken,et al.  Communication modality as a determinant of persuasion: The role of communicator salience. , 1983 .

[212]  C. Judd,et al.  The communication of social stereotypes: the effects of group discussion and information distribution on stereotypic appraisals. , 2001, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[213]  Mark P. Zanna,et al.  Cross-Dimension Ambivalence toward Social Groups: Can Ambivalence Affect Intentions to Hire Feminists? , 1998 .

[214]  Frank R. Kardes,et al.  On the automatic activation of attitudes. , 1986 .

[215]  D. Shanks Forward and Backward Blocking in Human Contingency Judgement , 1985 .

[216]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Why there are complementary learning systems in the hippocampus and neocortex: insights from the successes and failures of connectionist models of learning and memory. , 1995, Psychological review.

[217]  S. Moscovici,et al.  Biased information search in group decision making. , 2000, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[218]  P. Thagard,et al.  Explanatory coherence , 1993 .

[219]  N. Gilbert,et al.  Artificial Societies: The Computer Simulation of Social Life , 1995 .

[220]  Michael A. Arbib,et al.  The handbook of brain theory and neural networks , 1995, A Bradford book.

[221]  James Kennedy,et al.  Minds and Cultures: Particle Swarm Implications For Beings in Sociocognitive Space , 1999, Adapt. Behav..

[222]  R. Axelrod The Dissemination of Culture , 1997 .

[223]  B. Weiner,et al.  Spontaneous" causal thinking. , 1985 .

[224]  Arie W. Kruglanski,et al.  Covariation, discounting, and augmentation: Towards a clarification of attributional principles , 1978 .

[225]  Norbert Schwarz,et al.  Are (some) reports of attitude strength context dependent , 1996 .

[226]  David R. Shanks,et al.  Acquisition functions in contingency judgment , 1987 .

[227]  T. K. Srull,et al.  Handbook of Social Cognition , 1993 .

[228]  Dirk Van Rooy,et al.  A recurrent connectionist model of group biases. , 2003, Psychological review.

[229]  J. R. Larson,et al.  Diagnosing groups : Charting the flow of information in medical decision-making teams , 1996 .

[230]  S. Read,et al.  Explanatory coherence in social explanations : a parallel distributed processing account , 1993 .

[231]  G. Stasser,et al.  Expert role assignment and information sampling during collective recall and decision making. , 1995, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[232]  Lucian Gideon Conway,et al.  Influence of Impression-Management Goals on the Emerging Contents of Group Stereotypes: Support for a Social-Evolutionary Process , 1999 .

[233]  G. Bower,et al.  Evaluating an adaptive network model of human learning , 1988 .

[234]  D. Shanks Is Human Learning Rational? , 1995, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[235]  S. Chaiken,et al.  The psychology of attitudes. , 1993 .

[236]  Frank Van Overwalle,et al.  A Recurrent Connectionist Model of Person Impression Formation , 2004, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.

[237]  L. Allan Human contingency judgments: rule based or associative? , 1993, Psychological bulletin.

[238]  Brian Hazlehurst,et al.  The Emergence of Propositions from the Co-ordination of Talk and Action in a Shared World , 1998 .

[239]  R. Petty,et al.  Positive Mood and Persuasion: Different Roles for Affect Under High- and Low-Elaboration Conditions , 1993 .

[240]  D. Shanks,et al.  Human instrumental learning: a critical review of data and theory. , 1993, British journal of psychology.

[241]  M. Heesacker,et al.  Field dependence and attitude change: Source credibility can alter persuasion by affecting message-relevant thinking , 1983 .

[242]  Eliot R. Smith What do connectionism and social psychology offer each other , 1996 .

[243]  Michael I. Posner,et al.  Attention as a Cognitive and Neural System , 1992 .

[244]  Geoffrey E. Hinton,et al.  Distributed Representations , 1986, The Philosophy of Artificial Intelligence.

[245]  I. Couzin,et al.  Effective leadership and decision-making in animal groups on the move , 2005, Nature.

[246]  F. Strack,et al.  Reflective and Impulsive Determinants of Social Behavior , 2004, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.

[247]  J. Pearce Similarity and Discrimination: A Selective Review and a Connectionist Model , 1994 .