Targeting regions of interest for the study of the illiterate brain

This paper reviews a work project that uses illiteracy as a tool to understand the way the brain adapts to information. The project follows the exploration of certain targets that can be identified with the functions of reading and writing, both from the functional and from the anatomical points of view. Results concerning visual processing, cross‐modal operations (audiovisual and visuotactile), and interhemispheric crossing of information are reported. Studies with magnetoencephalography, with positron emission tomography, and with functional magnetic resonance provided evidence that the absence of school attendance at the usual age constitutes a handicap for the development of certain biological processes that serve behavioural functioning. Differences between groups of literate and illiterate subjects were found in several areas: while dealing with phonology a complex pattern of brain activation was only present in literate subjects; the corpus callosum in the segment where the parietal lobe fibres cro...

[1]  A. Tzavaras,et al.  Literacy and hemispheric specialization for language: Digit dichotic listening in illiterates , 1981, Neuropsychologia.

[2]  D. Wechsler A Standardized Memory Scale for Clinical Use , 1945 .

[3]  J. Morais,et al.  Ear differences in illiterates , 1987, Neuropsychologia.

[4]  Esmeralda Matute de Duran Aphasia in illiterates , 1986, Journal of Neurolinguistics.

[5]  M Ingvar,et al.  The illiterate brain. Learning to read and write during childhood influences the functional organization of the adult brain. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[6]  A. Castro-Caldas,et al.  Age and type of crossed aphasia in dextrals due to stroke , 1984, Brain and Language.

[7]  Gérard Deloche,et al.  A Calculation and Number Processing Battery for Clinical Application in Illiterates and Semi-Literates , 1999, Cortex.

[8]  A. Lecours,et al.  Illiteracy and brain damage 3: A contribution to the study of speech and language disorders in illiterates with unilateral brain damage (initial testing) , 1988, Neuropsychologia.

[9]  J. Matarazzo,et al.  The influence of age, education, IQ, gender, and alcohol abuse on Halstead-Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery performance. , 1989, Journal of clinical psychology.

[10]  A. Castro-Caldas,et al.  Neuropsychological Aspects of Illiteracy , 1997 .

[11]  Yale E. Cohen,et al.  A common reference frame for movement plans in the posterior parietal cortex , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[12]  Harold Goodglass,et al.  Normative data on the boston diagnostic aphasia examination, parietal lobe battery, and the boston naming Test , 1980 .

[13]  C. Papagno,et al.  Phonological Short-term Memory and the Learning of Novel Words: The Effect of Phonological Similarity and Item Length , 1992 .

[14]  Dominique Cardebat,et al.  The effects of age, educational background and sex on confrontation naming in normals; principles for testing naming ability , 1991 .

[15]  J. Morais,et al.  Finding Parts within Figures: A Developmental Study , 1987, Perception.

[16]  T. Robinson,et al.  Brain Plasticity and Behavior , 2003, Annual review of psychology.

[17]  A. Castro-Caldas,et al.  Non-verbal disturbances in crossed aphasia , 1987 .

[18]  A. Castro-Caldas,et al.  Influence of learning to read and write on the morphology of the corpus callosum , 1999, European journal of neurology.

[19]  A. Yamadori,et al.  Pure somaesthetic alexia: somaesthetic-verbal disconnection for letters. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[20]  M. Critchley Premorbid Literacy, and the Pattern of Subsequent Aphasia [Résumé] , 1956, Proceedings of the Royal Society of Medicine.

[21]  Monica Rosselli,et al.  Neuropsychological assessment in illiterates II. Language and praxic abilities , 1990, Brain and Cognition.

[22]  R. Bornstein,et al.  Educational level and neuropsychological performance in healthy elderly subjects , 1988 .

[23]  A. Scheibel,et al.  A quantitative dendritic analysis of wernicke's area in humans. II. Gender, hemispheric, and environmental factors , 1993, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[24]  A. Ardila,et al.  Sociocultural effects in neuropsychological assessment. , 1985, The International journal of neuroscience.

[25]  Yaakov Stern,et al.  Increased risk of mortality in alzheimer's disease patients with more advanced educational and occupational attainment , 1995, Annals of neurology.

[26]  A. Wechsler,et al.  Crossed aphasia in an illiterate dextral , 1976, Brain and Language.

[27]  A D Baddeley,et al.  The Children's Test of Nonword Repetition: a test of phonological working memory. , 1994, Memory.

[28]  A. Castro-Caldas,et al.  Illiteracy: A cause for biased cognitive development , 1997, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[29]  R D Currier,et al.  Aphasia and literacy. , 1971, The British journal of disorders of communication.

[30]  Hanna Damasio,et al.  Reversal of ear advantage for phonetically similar words in illiterates , 1979 .

[31]  G. Dellatolas,et al.  Assessment of Calculation and Number Processing by Adults: Cognitive and Neuropsychological Issues , 1999, Perceptual and motor skills.

[32]  Karl Magnus Petersson,et al.  Language Processing Modulated by Literacy: A Network Analysis of Verbal Repetition in Literate and Illiterate Subjects , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[33]  D. Aram,et al.  SCHOLASTIC APTITUDE AND ACHIEVEMENT AMONG CHILDREN WITH UNILATERAL BRAIN LESIONS , 1987, Pediatric Research.

[34]  A. Ardila,et al.  Effects of cultural background and education on handedness , 1989, Neuropsychologia.

[35]  L. Vygotsky Mind in Society: The Development of Higher Psychological Processes: Harvard University Press , 1978 .

[36]  M. Rosselli Neuropsychology of illiteracy. , 1993, Behavioural neurology.

[37]  I. Martins,et al.  Acquired procedural dyscalculia associated to a left parietal lesion in a child. , 1999, Child neuropsychology : a journal on normal and abnormal development in childhood and adolescence.

[38]  A. Damasio,et al.  Brain Specialization for Language: Not Dependent on Literacy-Reply , 1976 .

[39]  A Reis,et al.  Influence of educational level of non brain-damaged subjects on visual naming capacities. , 1994, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[40]  R. Currier,et al.  Brain Specialization for Language: Not Dependent on Literacy , 1976 .

[41]  J. G. Snodgrass,et al.  A standardized set of 260 pictures: norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. Human learning and memory.

[42]  Karl Magnus Petersson,et al.  Effective Auditory–Verbal Encoding Activates the Left Prefrontal and the Medial Temporal Lobes: A Generalization to Illiterate Subjects , 1999, NeuroImage.

[43]  A. Robles,et al.  Does literacy have an effect on stick construction tasks? , 2000, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[44]  P. Bertelson,et al.  Does awareness of speech as a sequence of phones arise spontaneously? , 1979, Cognition.

[45]  D. J. Murray Articulation and acoustic confusability in short-term memory. , 1968 .

[46]  A. Ardila,et al.  Neuropsychological test performance in illiterate subjects. , 1998, Archives of clinical neuropsychology : the official journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists.

[47]  Karl Magnus Petersson,et al.  Formal Schooling Influences Two- but Not Three-Dimensional Naming Skills , 2001, Brain and Cognition.

[48]  A. Ardila,et al.  Neuropsychological Evaluation of the Spanish Speaker , 1994, Critical Issues in Neuropsychology.

[49]  Monica Rosselli,et al.  Neuropsychological assessment in illiterates: Visuospatial and memory abilities , 1989, Brain and Cognition.

[50]  Jacques Mehler,et al.  Illiteracy and brain damage—1. Aphasia testing in culturally contrasted populations (control subjects) , 1987, Neuropsychologia.

[51]  A. Damasio,et al.  Brain specialization for language does not depend on literacy. , 1976, Archives of neurology.