Discrimination learning and reversal of the conditioned eyeblink reflex in a rodent model of autism
暂无分享,去创建一个
Kevin L. Brown | Mark E. Stanton | M. Stanton | K. Brown | P. Rodier | E. Peloso | Patricia Rodier | Elizabeth Peloso
[1] A. Philippi,et al. Haplotypes in the gene encoding protein kinase c-beta (PRKCB1) on chromosome 16 are associated with autism , 2005, Molecular Psychiatry.
[2] R. F. Thompson,et al. Cerebellum: essential involvement in the classically conditioned eyelid response. , 1984, Science.
[3] J. Coldren,et al. Spatial Reversal as a Measure of Executive Functioning in Children With Autism , 2003, The Journal of genetic psychology.
[4] E. Engle,et al. Homozygous HOXA1 mutations disrupt human brainstem, inner ear, cardiovascular and cognitive development , 2005, Nature Genetics.
[5] J. Herbert,et al. Contrasts in Infant Classical Eyeblink Conditioning as a Function of Premature Birth , 2004 .
[6] Richard F. Thompson,et al. Impaired motor coordination correlates with persistent multiple climbing fiber innervation in PKCγ mutant mice , 1995, Cell.
[7] M. Stanton,et al. Eyeblink conditioning in the developing rat. , 1992, Behavioral Neuroscience.
[8] T. Jernigan,et al. Abnormal development of the cerebellar vermis in children prenatally exposed to alcohol: size reduction in lobules I-V. , 1996, Alcoholism, clinical and experimental research.
[9] P. Rodier,et al. Converging evidence for brain stem injury in autism , 2002, Development and Psychopathology.
[10] M. Stanton,et al. Alcohol-Induced Damage to the Developing Brain: Functional Approaches Using Classical Eyeblink Conditioning , 2002 .
[11] M. Stanton,et al. Neonatal alcohol exposure impairs acquisition of eyeblink conditioned responses during discrimination learning and reversal in weanling rats. , 2007, Developmental psychobiology.
[12] M. Stanton,et al. Ontogeny of delay versus trace eyeblink conditioning in the rat. , 2000, Developmental psychobiology.
[13] S. Bryson,et al. Minor malformations and physical measurements in autism: data from Nova Scotia. , 1997, Teratology.
[14] M. Allin,et al. Cognitive and motor function and the size of the cerebellum in adolescents born very pre-term. , 2001, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[15] B. Pennington,et al. Executive functions in young children with autism. , 1999, Child development.
[16] D. Nicholson,et al. Developmental changes in the neural mechanisms of eyeblink conditioning. , 2004, Behavioral and cognitive neuroscience reviews.
[17] R. Skelton,et al. Bilateral cerebellar lesions disrupt conditioned eyelid responses in unrestrained rats. , 1988, Behavioral neuroscience.
[18] M. Stanton,et al. Developmental Studies of Eyeblink Conditioning in the Rat , 2002 .
[19] J. Steinmetz,et al. Abnormal classical eye-blink conditioning in autism , 1994, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.
[20] M. Stanton,et al. Ontogeny of eyeblink conditioning using a visual conditional stimulus. , 1999, Developmental psychobiology.
[21] P. Rodier,et al. Prenatal exposure of rats to valproic acid reproduces the cerebellar anomalies associated with autism. , 2000, Neurotoxicology and teratology.
[22] John T. Green. The effects of ethanol on the developing cerebellum and eyeblink classical conditioning , 2008, The Cerebellum.
[23] J. Steinmetz,et al. Eyeblink Classical Conditioning: Volume 2 , 2002 .
[24] J. Steinmetz. Brain substrates of classical eyeblink conditioning: a highly localized but also distributed system , 2000, Behavioural Brain Research.
[25] C. Gillberg,et al. Autism associated with conditions characterized by developmental errors in early embryogenesis: a mini review , 2005, International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience.
[26] M. Stanton,et al. Effects of Early Hippocampal Lesions on Trace, Delay, and Long-Delay Eyeblink Conditioning in Developing Rats , 2001, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.
[27] M. Stanton,et al. Eyeblink conditioning in the infant rat: an animal model of learning in developmental neurotoxicology. , 1994, Environmental health perspectives.
[28] P. Solomon,et al. Altered activity in the hippocampus is more detrimental to classical conditioning than removing the structure. , 1983, Science.
[29] M. Stanton. Multiple memory systems, development and conditioning , 2000, Behavioural Brain Research.
[30] G. Dawson,et al. Defining the broader phenotype of autism: Genetic, brain, and behavioral perspectives , 2002, Development and Psychopathology.
[31] P. Rodier,et al. The teratology of autism , 2005, International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience.
[32] P. Rodier,et al. Embryological origin for autism: Developmental anomalies of the cranial nerve motor nuclei , 1996, The Journal of comparative neurology.
[33] M. Stanton,et al. Disruption of cerebellar maturation by an antimitotic agent impairs the ontogeny of eyeblink conditioning in rats , 1995 .
[34] E. W. Simon,et al. One-year longitudinal investigation of eyeblink classical conditioning and cognitive and behavioral tests in adults with down's syndrome , 1994 .
[35] Richard F. Thompson,et al. Neural substrates of eyeblink conditioning: acquisition and retention. , 2003, Learning & memory.
[36] Alessandro Quattrone,et al. Association between the HOXA1 A218G polymorphism and increased head circumference in patients with autism , 2004, Biological Psychiatry.
[37] Richard E Thompson,et al. Cerebellar circuits and synaptic mechanisms involved in classical eyeblink conditioning , 1997, Trends in Neurosciences.
[38] L. E. Ross,et al. Acquisition and differential conditioning of the eyelid response in normal and retarded children. , 1968, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[39] J. Steinmetz,et al. Animal Models in Eyeblink Classical Conditioning , 2002 .
[40] M. Stanton,et al. Neonatal ethanol exposure impairs eyeblink conditioning in weanling rats. , 1998, Alcoholism, clinical and experimental research.
[41] Joseph E. Steinmetz,et al. Abnormal classical eyeblink conditioning in autism , 1993 .
[42] P. Turnpenny,et al. A clinical study of 57 children with fetal anticonvulsant syndromes , 2000 .
[43] J. Herbert,et al. The ontogeny of human learning in delay, long-delay, and trace eyeblink conditioning. , 2003, Behavioral neuroscience.
[44] J. Steinmetz,et al. Classical Eyeblink Conditioning in Normal and Autistic Children , 2002 .
[45] G. Dawson,et al. Performance on Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery Subtests Sensitive to Frontal Lobe Function in People with Autistic Disorder: Evidence from the Collaborative Programs of Excellence in Autism Network , 2004, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.
[46] P. Turnpenny,et al. Characteristics of fetal anticonvulsant syndrome associated autistic disorder. , 2005, Developmental medicine and child neurology.
[47] W. B. Orr,et al. Hippocampectomy selectively disrupts discrimination reversal conditioning of the rabbit nictitating membrane response , 1983, Behavioural Brain Research.
[48] T. Berger,et al. Hippocampal lesions disrupt classical conditioning of cross-modality reversal learning of the rabbit nictitating membrane response , 1986, Behavioural Brain Research.
[49] D. Powell,et al. Both medial prefrontal and amygdala central nucleus lesions abolish heart rate classical conditioning, but only prefrontal lesions impair reversal of eyeblink differential conditioning , 1998, Neuroscience Letters.