Applying Eye Tracking to Identify Autism Spectrum Disorder in Children

Eye tracking (ET) holds potential for the early detection of autism spectrum disorder (ASD). To overcome the difficulties of working with young children, developing a short and informative paradigm is crucial for ET. We investigated the fixation times of 37 ASD and 37 typically developing (TD) children ages 4–6 watching a 10-second video of a female speaking. ASD children showed significant reductions in fixation time at six areas of interest. Furthermore, discriminant analysis revealed fixation times at the mouth and body could significantly discriminate ASD from TD with a classification accuracy of 85.1%, sensitivity of 86.5%, and specificity of 83.8%. Our study suggests that a short video clip may provide enough information to distinguish ASD from TD children.

[1]  Katarzyna Chawarska,et al.  Looking But Not Seeing: Atypical Visual Scanning and Recognition of Faces in 2 and 4-Year-Old Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder , 2009, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[2]  Sven Bölte,et al.  Eye tracking in early autism research , 2013, Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders.

[3]  Li Yi,et al.  Identifying children with autism spectrum disorder based on their face processing abnormality: A machine learning framework , 2016, Autism research : official journal of the International Society for Autism Research.

[4]  Michael Murias,et al.  The Role of Face Familiarity in Eye Tracking of Faces by Individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorders , 2008, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[5]  L. Lee,et al.  Screening for Autism with the SRS and SCQ: Variations across Demographic, Developmental and Behavioral Factors in Preschool Children , 2017, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[6]  C. Gillberg,et al.  Gaze Performance in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder when Observing Communicative Actions , 2012, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[7]  Florian B. Pokorny,et al.  How can clinicians detect and treat autism early? Methodological trends of technology use in research , 2016, Acta paediatrica.

[8]  Kim M. Dalton,et al.  Gaze fixation and the neural circuitry of face processing in autism , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[9]  Katarzyna Chawarska,et al.  Speech Disturbs Face Scanning in 6-Month-Old Infants Who Develop Autism Spectrum Disorder , 2014, Biological Psychiatry.

[10]  L. Adamson,et al.  The Utility of the Social Communication Questionnaire in Screening for Autism in Children Referred for Early Intervention , 2007 .

[11]  A. Scarpa,et al.  The Modified Checklist for Autism in Toddlers: Reliability in a Diverse Rural American Sample , 2013, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders.

[12]  P. Biondich,et al.  Identifying Infants and Young Children With Developmental Disorders in the Medical Home: An Algorithm for Developmental Surveillance and Screening , 2006, Pediatrics.

[13]  A. Crippa,et al.  An Integrated Model of Executive Functioning is Helpful for Understanding ADHD and Associated Disorders , 2015, Journal of attention disorders.

[14]  M. Falkmer,et al.  Diagnostic procedures in autism spectrum disorders: a systematic literature review , 2013, European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry.

[15]  Natacha Akshoomoff,et al.  The Role of the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule in the Assessment of Autism Spectrum Disorders in School and Community Settings , 2006, The California school psychologist : CASP.

[16]  D. Wall,et al.  Searching for a minimal set of behaviors for autism detection through feature selection-based machine learning , 2015, Translational Psychiatry.

[17]  Lonnie Zwaigenbaum,et al.  Autism spectrum disorder: advances in diagnosis and evaluation , 2018, British Medical Journal.

[18]  A. E. Cook,et al.  Face processing in children with autism , 2007, Autism : the international journal of research and practice.

[19]  A. Klin,et al.  Absence of preferential looking to the eyes of approaching adults predicts level of social disability in 2-year-old toddlers with autism spectrum disorder. , 2008, Archives of general psychiatry.

[20]  Simon Baron-Cohen,et al.  Autism , 2008, The Lancet.

[21]  Young Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder Do Not Preferentially Attend to Biological Motion , 2012, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[22]  F. Tarazi,et al.  Autism Spectrum Disorder: Classification, diagnosis and therapy , 2018, Pharmacology & therapeutics.

[23]  P. Strand,et al.  A Review of Level 2 Parent-Report Instruments Used to Screen Children Aged 1.5–5 for Autism: A Meta-Analytic Update , 2015, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[24]  Lauren E. Libero,et al.  Identification of neural connectivity signatures of autism using machine learning , 2013, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[25]  A. Klin,et al.  Two-year-olds with autism orient to nonsocial contingencies rather than biological motion , 2009, Nature.

[26]  H. Kosaka,et al.  Visual attention for social information and salivary oxytocin levels in preschool children with autism spectrum disorders: an eye-tracking study , 2014, Front. Neurosci..

[27]  Mark H. Johnson,et al.  Autism and the Social Brain: The First-Year Puzzle , 2016, Biological Psychiatry.

[28]  Eric W. Klingemier,et al.  A Meta-Analysis of Gaze Differences to Social and Nonsocial Information Between Individuals With and Without Autism. , 2017, Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry.

[29]  Yuko Okamoto,et al.  Gazefinder as a clinical supplementary tool for discriminating between autism spectrum disorder and typical development in male adolescents and adults , 2016, Molecular Autism.

[30]  C. Delahunty Developmental delays and autism: Screening and surveillance , 2015, Cleveland Clinic Journal of Medicine.

[31]  Joel Park,et al.  Introduction of a New Video-Based Eye Tracking Paradigm for Early Detection of ASD , 2017 .