The Universal Constraint Set: Convention, not Fact

All languages make the same phonological generalisations. This is the remarkable claim of Optimality Theory (OT). In early generative phonology (Chomsky & Halle 1968), phonological gen-eralisations were expressed by ordered rewrite rules. Each language, however, required its own set of rules as well as its own ordering. Later, underspeci-cation phonology (Archangeli & Pulleyblank 1989, 1994) emphasised default rules. Universal tendencies in the rules were apparent, but characterising all languages with a single set of rules remained an unreachable dream. In OT, phonological generalisations are expressed as ranked defeasible constraints. Ranking provides so many distinct but plausible grammars that it seems feasible that a universal set of phonological generalisations could account for the diversity of phonological systems. The question we face is no longer whether the assumption of such a universal set is theoretically tenable, but whether it is justiiable. There are two senses in which such an assumption could be justiied: either as a fact or as a convention. If a fact, it claims that all language users objectively instantiate the same set of generalisations. If a convention, it encourages phonologists to describe languages using an agreed but arbitrary system of gen-eralisations. In this interpretation, the universal constraint set is as arbitrary, but as useful, as the international phonetic alphabet (IPA). This chapter examines seven kinds of argument for one or other status of the universality of phonological constraints. These are the arguments from empirical evidence (section 2), restrictiveness (section 3), simplicity (section 4), universal markedness, acquisition (both section 5), learnability (section 6), and convention (section 7). Close examination nds all but the last of these arguments to be wanting. The conclusion that remains is that universality, like the IPA, makes a better convention than fact. It should be used rather than believed. Optimality Theory (Prince & Smolensky 1993, for an introduction see Archangeli & Langendoen 1997) is rst and most frequently applied to phonology 1. In this I would like to thank the following people for their comments on an earlier version of this paper: and one anonymous reviewer. I would also like to thank the editors for their patience and interest. 1 The formalism has also been applied to morphology (e.

[1]  Letitia R. Naigles,et al.  Learnability and Cognition: The Acquisition of Argument Structure , 1991 .

[2]  J. Rissanen,et al.  Modeling By Shortest Data Description* , 1978, Autom..

[3]  D. Stampe A dissertation on natural phonology , 1979 .

[4]  Richard Wiese,et al.  Zero morphology and constraint interaction: subtraction and epenthesis in German dialects , 1996 .

[5]  Peter D. Moore,et al.  Reconstructing the past , 1979, Nature.

[6]  N. Chater,et al.  Computational models and Rethinking innateness , 1999, Journal of Child Language.

[7]  Virginia Valian,et al.  Logical and Psychological Constraints on the Acquisition of Syntax , 1990 .

[8]  Alan S. Prince,et al.  Faithfulness and reduplicative identity , 1995 .

[9]  Ellen Woolford,et al.  Object Agreement in Palauan: Specificity, Humanness, Economy and Optimality , 2000 .

[10]  Steven Bird,et al.  Phonology , 2002, A Grammar of Patwin.

[11]  E. Matthei,et al.  Crossing boundaries: more evidence for phonological constraints on early multi-word utterances , 1989, Journal of Child Language.

[12]  J. Woodward,et al.  Scientific Explanation and the Causal Structure of the World , 1988 .

[13]  T. Mark Ellison,et al.  Induction and inherent similarity , 2001, Similarity and Categorization.

[14]  Bruce Tesar,et al.  Learnability in Optimality Theory (long version) , 1996 .

[15]  Laura Benua Identity Effects in Morphological Truncation , 1995 .

[16]  C. S. Wallace,et al.  Single-factor analysis by minimum message length estimation , 1992 .

[17]  Steven Bird,et al.  Computational phonology: A constraint-based approach , 1995, CL.

[18]  C. A. Ferguson,et al.  WORDS AND SOUNDS IN EARLY LANGUAGE ACQUISITION , 1975 .

[19]  C. S. Wallace,et al.  An Information Measure for Classification , 1968, Comput. J..

[20]  Gilbert Harman Two quibbles about analyticity and psychological reality , 1980, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[21]  Ming Li,et al.  Inductive Reasoning and Kolmogorov Complexity , 1992, J. Comput. Syst. Sci..

[22]  E. W. Strong Newton's Philosophy of Nature; Selections from his Writings , 1955 .

[23]  Steven Bird,et al.  One-Level Phonology: Autosegmental Representations and Rules as Finite Automata , 1994, Comput. Linguistics.

[24]  E. Clark Awareness of Language: Some Evidence from what Children Say and Do , 1978 .

[25]  P. Fikkert Acquisition of phonology , 1995 .

[26]  William D. Raymond,et al.  An Optimality-Theoretic Typology of Case and Grammatical Voice Systems , 1993 .

[27]  Diana Archangeli,et al.  Yoruba vowel harmony , 1989 .

[28]  P. Smolensky,et al.  The Learnability of Optimality Theory: An Algorithm and Some Basic Complexity Results , 1995 .

[29]  J. Rissanen Estimation of structure by minimum description length , 1982 .

[30]  L. Elton,et al.  THE DIRECTION OF TIME , 1978 .

[31]  Bruce Tesar,et al.  Computational optimality theory , 1996 .

[32]  Noam Chomsky,et al.  The Sound Pattern of English , 1968 .

[33]  James M. Scobbie,et al.  Attribute value phonology , 1991 .

[34]  Peter Sells,et al.  An optimality theoretic approach to variation in negative inversion in AAVE , 1996 .

[35]  James M Scobbie,et al.  Key aspects of declarative phonology , 1996 .

[36]  R. Jakobson Child Language, Aphasia and Phonological Universals , 1980 .

[37]  C. Snow,et al.  Input and interaction in language acquisition: The changing role of negative evidence in theories of language development , 1994 .

[38]  K. Russell Morphemes and candidates in Optimality Theory , 1995 .

[39]  J. McCarthy Extensions of faithfulness: Rotuman revisited , 1995 .

[40]  Ming Li,et al.  An Introduction to Kolmogorov Complexity and Its Applications , 1997, Texts in Computer Science.

[41]  E. Mark Gold,et al.  Language Identification in the Limit , 1967, Inf. Control..

[42]  Kimmo Koskenniemi,et al.  Two-Level Model for Morphological Analysis , 1983, IJCAI.

[43]  T. Sejnowski,et al.  Irresistible environment meets immovable neurons , 1997, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[44]  P. Smolensky The Initial State and 'Richness of the Base' in Optimality Theory , 1996 .

[45]  L. Leonard,et al.  Do children pick and choose? an examination of phonological selection and avoidance in early lexical acquisition , 1982, Journal of Child Language.

[46]  S. Pinker The Language Instinct , 1994 .