BY CURT P. RICHTER Psychobiological Laboratory, Phipps Psychiatric Clinic, Johns Hopkins University O NE of the most fundamental of all the phenomena which characterize animal life and distinguish it from plant life is the spontaneous motility of the animal organism. A few plants, to be sure, especially certain forms of marine vegetation, do move about, but these few are exceptions in the plant kingdom. The activity of animals, on the other hand, although it varies widely in form and extent from species to species, is an ordinary phenomenon which one always anticipates under normal circumstances. We may ask, then, what it is that sets off the diverse performances which animals display. Ordinarily we think of most of their activity as being due to some form of external stimulation. We know, however, that all animals, from the lowest. uni-cellular organism to man, are active even when all external stimuli have been eliminated. And since this spontaneous motility, just as any other kind of motility, must have a definite cause, it must be due to some natural factor within the organism. Many workers have chosen to call it "voluntary" activity, presumably because of the common belief that the "will" to do is the origin of the ction. We believe, however, that spontaneous activity arises from certain underlying physiological origins. We shall attempt to show from studies chiefly on the white rat what some of these origins are, and how they fit into the general biological picture of the animal's life. The investigations described below have been made by Ging H. Wang, Elaine F. Kinder, Tomi Wada, and the present writer in the Psychobiological Laboratory of the Phipps Psychiatric Clinic during the past six years. Some of the experiments have already been reported elsewhere, but we have taken this opportunity to collect also numerous observations that are as yet unpublished. Of the extensive work on animal "drives" done by Hoskins (i92.5), Moss (I924), Slonaker (I924, I92X5, I92.6), Stone (I92.4, I92.5, I92.6), Szymanski (i92.0, i92.2.), and Tracy (i92.6) we shall incorporate in this review only that part which bears directly on our own method of approach or on our own experimental findings.
[1]
E. Kinder.
A study of the nest‐building activity of the albino rat
,
1927
.
[2]
E. Bugbee,et al.
THE INCREASE OF VOLUNTARY ACTIVITY OF OVARIECTOMIZED ALBINO RATS CAUSED BY INJECTIONS OF OVARIAN FOLLICULAR HORMONE
,
1926
.
[3]
H. C. Tracy,et al.
The development of motility and behavior reactions in the toadfish (Opsanus tau)
,
1926
.
[4]
F. Rogers,et al.
X-RAY OBSERVATIONS OF HUNGER CONTRACTIONS IN MAN
,
1926
.
[5]
C. P. Stone.
PRELIMINARY NOTE ON THE MATERNAL BEHAVIOR OF RATS LIVING IN PARABIOSIS
,
1925
.
[6]
R. G. Hoskins.
STUDIES ON VIGOR. IV. THE EFFECT OF TESTICLE GRAFTS ON SPONTANEOUS ACTIVITY
,
1925
.
[7]
J. Slonaker.
THE EFFECT OF PUBESCENCE, OESTRUATION AND MENOPAUSE ON THE VOLUNTARY ACTIVITY IN THE ALBINO RAT
,
1924
.
[8]
A. J. Carlson.
The Control of Hunger in Health and Disease
,
1916,
The Psychological Clinic.
[9]
J. Angell.
Bodily Changes in Pain, Hunger, Fear and Rage; An Account of Recent Researches into the Function of Emotional Excitement
,
1915
.
[10]
C. P. Stone.
The initial copulatory response of female rats reared in isolation from the age of twenty days to the age of puberty.
,
1926
.
[11]
F. Moss.
Study of Animal Drives.
,
1924
.
[12]
G. H. Wang.
The relation between "spontaneous" activity and oestrous cycle in the white rat
,
1923
.
[13]
Herbert M. Evans,et al.
The oestrous cycle in the rat and its associated phenomena
,
1922
.
[14]
Curt P. Richter,et al.
A behavioristic study of the activity of the rat
,
1922
.
[15]
C. Martin.
Thermal adjustment and respiratory exchange in monotremes and mersupials.—A study in the development of homothermism
,
1901,
Proceedings of the Royal Society of London.