Internal model of gravity for hand interception: parametric adaptation to zero-gravity visual targets on Earth.

Internal model is a neural mechanism that mimics the dynamics of an object for sensory motor or cognitive functions. Recent research focuses on the issue of whether multiple internal models are learned and switched to cope with a variety of conditions, or single general models are adapted by tuning the parameters. Here we addressed this issue by investigating how the manual interception of a moving target changes with changes of the visual environment. In our paradigm, a virtual target moves vertically downward on a screen with different laws of motion. Subjects are asked to punch a hidden ball that arrives in synchrony with the visual target. By using several different protocols, we systematically found that subjects do not develop a new internal model appropriate for constant speed targets, but they use the default gravity model and reduce the central processing time. The results imply that adaptation to zero-gravity targets involves a compression of temporal processing through the cortical and subcortical regions interconnected with the vestibular cortex, which has previously been shown to be the site of storage of the internal model of gravity.

[1]  Simon K. Rushton,et al.  Weighted combination of size and disparity: a computational model for timing a ball catch , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[2]  F. Lacquaniti,et al.  The role of preparation in tuning anticipatory and reflex responses during catching , 1989, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[3]  F. Lacquaniti,et al.  Internal models of target motion: expected dynamics overrides measured kinematics in timing manual interceptions. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[4]  F. A. Mussa-Ivaldi,et al.  Does the motor control system use multiple models and context switching to cope with a variable environment? , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[5]  R. Shepard Ecological constraints on internal representation: resonant kinematics of perceiving, imagining, thinking, and dreaming. , 1984, Psychological review.

[6]  Anne-Marie Brouwer,et al.  Hitting moving targets , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[7]  Zoubin Ghahramani,et al.  Perspectives and problems in motor learning , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[8]  J. Smeets,et al.  When is behavioral data evidence for a control theory? Tau-coupling revisited. , 2003, Motor control.

[9]  A. Georgopoulos Catching for real and catching for fun in ecological psychology. Focus on "Internal models of target motion: expected dynamics overrides measured kinematics in timing manual interceptions". , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[10]  C. Michaels,et al.  Information and action in punching a falling ball , 2001, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[11]  Apostolos P Georgopoulos,et al.  Cognitive motor control: spatial and temporal aspects , 2003, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[12]  David N. Lee,et al.  Visual Timing in Hitting An Accelerating Ball , 1983, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[13]  R. Hari,et al.  Cortical control of human motoneuron firing during isometric contraction. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[14]  Daniel Bullock,et al.  Prospective control of manual interceptive actions: comparative simulations of extant and new model constructs , 2001, Neural Networks.

[15]  D. Boussaoud,et al.  Parietal inputs to dorsal versus ventral premotor areas in the macaque monkey: evidence for largely segregated visuomotor pathways , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[16]  A. G. Witney,et al.  Predictive motor learning of temporal delays. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[17]  T. Hayton The Advanced Theory of Statistics, Vol. 3 , 1968 .

[18]  C. Prablanc,et al.  Automatic control during hand reaching at undetected two-dimensional target displacements. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.

[19]  J R Flanagan,et al.  The Role of Internal Models in Motion Planning and Control: Evidence from Grip Force Adjustments during Movements of Hand-Held Loads , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[20]  P. Cavanagh,et al.  Electromechanical delay in human skeletal muscle under concentric and eccentric contractions , 1979, European Journal of Applied Physiology and Occupational Physiology.

[21]  Apostolos P. Georgopoulos,et al.  Interception of real and apparent motion targets: psychophysics in humans and monkeys , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[22]  Daniel Bullock,et al.  How Position, Velocity, and Temporal Information Combine in the Prospective Control of Catching: Data and Model , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[23]  Daniel M. Wolpert,et al.  Internal models underlying grasp can be additively combined , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[24]  A. Georgopoulos,et al.  Neural responses in motor cortex and area 7a to real and apparent motion , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[25]  E. Brenner,et al.  Hitting moving targets Continuous control of the acceleration of the hand on the basis of the target’s velocity , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[26]  A. Georgopoulos,et al.  Neural responses during interception of real and apparent circularly moving stimuli in motor cortex and area 7a. , 2004, Cerebral cortex.

[27]  M. Grealy,et al.  Judging Time Intervals Using a Model of Perceptuo-Motor Control , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[28]  John W. Krakauer,et al.  Independent learning of internal models for kinematic and dynamic control of reaching , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[29]  J. Tresilian Visually timed action: time-out for ‘tau’? , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[30]  Arnaud Delorme,et al.  Spike-based strategies for rapid processing , 2001, Neural Networks.

[31]  D. Regan,et al.  Dissociation of discrimination thresholds for time to contact and for rate of angular expansion , 1993, Vision Research.

[32]  David N. Lee Guiding Movement by Coupling Taus , 1998 .

[33]  Martin Faint,et al.  Does the brain model newton’s laws? , 2001 .

[34]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Failure to Consolidate the Consolidation Theory of Learning for Sensorimotor Adaptation Tasks , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[35]  E Bizzi,et al.  Motor learning by field approximation. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[36]  Daeyeol Lee,et al.  Manual interception of moving targets I. Performance and movement initiation , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[37]  N. A. Borghese,et al.  Transient reversal of the stretch reflex in human arm muscles. , 1991, Journal of neurophysiology.

[38]  Marion A. Eppler,et al.  Development of Visually Guided Locomotion , 1998 .

[39]  M. Kendall,et al.  The advanced theory of statistics , 1945 .

[40]  M. Kawato,et al.  Modular organization of internal models of tools in the human cerebellum , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[41]  K. J. Cole,et al.  Memory representations underlying motor commands used during manipulation of common and novel objects. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[42]  J. Gibson The Senses Considered As Perceptual Systems , 1967 .

[43]  Nicholas L. Port,et al.  Motor Cortical Activity during Interception of Moving Targets. , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[44]  Francesco Lacquaniti,et al.  The role of vision in tuning anticipatory motor responses of the limbs , 1993 .

[45]  James R Tresilian,et al.  The Accuracy of Interceptive Action in Time and Space , 2004, Exercise and sport sciences reviews.

[46]  A. Berthoz Multisensory control of movement , 1993 .

[47]  Daeyeol Lee,et al.  Neuronal Clusters in the Primate Motor Cortex during Interceptin of Moving Targets. , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[48]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Posterior parietal cortex in rhesus monkey: I. Parcellation of areas based on distinctive limbic and sensory corticocortical connections , 1989, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[49]  A. G. Witney,et al.  Learning and decay of prediction in object manipulation. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[50]  M. G. Kendall,et al.  The advanced theory of statistics. Vols. 2. , 1969 .

[51]  W. T. Thach,et al.  Throwing while looking through prisms. II. Specificity and storage of multiple gaze-throw calibrations. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[52]  R. Christina,et al.  Psychology of motor behavior and sport , 1978 .

[53]  M. Kawato,et al.  Random presentation enables subjects to adapt to two opposing forces on the hand , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[54]  Paul B. Johnson,et al.  Cortical networks for visual reaching: physiological and anatomical organization of frontal and parietal lobe arm regions. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[55]  F. Lacquaniti,et al.  Anticipatory and reflex coactivation of antagonist muscles in catching , 1987, Brain Research.

[56]  R. Shepard Ecological constraints on internal representation: resonant kinematics of perceiving, imagining, thinking, and dreaming. , 1984, Psychological review.

[57]  F. Lacquaniti,et al.  Fast adaptation of the internal model of gravity for manual interceptions: evidence for event-dependent learning. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[58]  J. Winn,et al.  Brain , 1878, The Lancet.

[59]  R. Bootsma,et al.  On the information-based regulation of movement: What Wann (1996) may want to consider , 1997 .

[60]  J. Movshon,et al.  The Timing of Response Onset and Offset in Macaque Visual Neurons , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[61]  S. Thorpe,et al.  Surfing a spike wave down the ventral stream , 2002, Vision Research.

[62]  S. Thorpe,et al.  A Limit to the Speed of Processing in Ultra-Rapid Visual Categorization of Novel Natural Scenes , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[63]  N. A. Borghese,et al.  Internal models of limb geometry in the control of hand compliance , 1992, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[64]  Q. Mcnemar Note on the sampling error of the difference between correlated proportions or percentages , 1947, Psychometrika.

[65]  J R Tresilian,et al.  Four Questions of Time to Contact: A Critical Examination of Research on Interceptive Timing , 1993, Perception.

[66]  Konrad Paul Kording,et al.  Bayesian integration in sensorimotor learning , 2004, Nature.

[67]  Mitsuo Kawato,et al.  Internal models for motor control and trajectory planning , 1999, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[68]  F. Lacquaniti,et al.  Representation of Visual Gravitational Motion in the Human Vestibular Cortex , 2005, Science.

[69]  Anne-Marie Brouwer,et al.  Perception of acceleration with short presentation times: Can acceleration be used in interception? , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.

[70]  D M Wolpert,et al.  Multiple paired forward and inverse models for motor control , 1998, Neural Networks.

[71]  P. Werkhoven,et al.  Visual processing of optic acceleration , 1992, Vision Research.

[72]  N. A. Borghese,et al.  Time-varying mechanical behavior of multijointed arm in man. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.