PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article VISUAL PRIOR ENTRY

— It has long been claimed that attended stimuli are perceived prior to unattended stimuli—the doctrine of prior entry. Most, if not all, studies on which such claims have been based, however, are open to a nonattentional interpretation involving response bias, leading some researchers to assert that prior entry may not exist. Given this controversy, we introduce a novel methodology to minimize the effect of response bias by manipulating attention and response demands in orthogonal dimensions. Attention was oriented to the left or right (i.e., spatially), but instead of reporting on the basis of location, observers reported the order (first or second) of vertical versus horizontal line segments. Although second-order response biases were demonstrated, effects of attention in accordance with the law of prior entry were clearly obtained following both exogenous and endogenous attentional cuing. The doctrine of prior entry—that attended stimuli are perceived earlier than unattended stimuli—has a long history in experimental psychology, stretching back to the very origins of the study of perception Some of the most compelling evidence demonstrating that attention influences the perception of arrival times comes from recent studies using temporal-order judgment (TOJ) tasks (e. In a typical study, attention is oriented to the left or right of fixa-tion, and observers are required to indicate which of two subsequent stimuli, one presented on the left and one on the right, was presented first. In most studies, the interval between the two stimuli is varied, and the point of subjective simultaneity (PSS) is determined for each type of cue. The PSS represents the interval for which the observer perceives the stimuli as simultaneous and is computed as the interval at which " left first " and " right first " responses are reported equally often ; that is, the PSS is the point at which observers are maximally uncertain as to the correct response. The accelerative effect of attention that is presumed by prior entry is indicated by a shift in the PSS. We believe genuine prior entry should be defined as a perceptual effect on arrival times attributable to attentional modulation (cf. Titchener, 1908) once the opportunities and incentives for response-and decision-level contributions to the measured PSS have been minimized. One such postperceptual mechanism that has recently been highlighted A simple response-bias account of the shift in PSS postulates that observers may simply report the side to which they had been instructed to attend (Frey, …

[1]  C. Spence,et al.  Multisensory prior entry. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[2]  W. Schmidt,et al.  Endogenous attention and illusory line motion reexamined. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[3]  J. Stolz Word recognition and temporal order judgments: semantics turns back the clock. , 1999, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.

[4]  J T Enns,et al.  The duration of a brief event in the mind's eye. , 1999, The Journal of general psychology.

[5]  J. Mattingley,et al.  Phasic alerting of neglect patients overcomes their spatial deficit in visual awareness , 1998, Nature.

[6]  K. Briand,et al.  Feature integration and spatial attention : More evidence of a dissociation between endogenous and exogenous orienting , 1998 .

[7]  Jüri Allik,et al.  Multiple Visual Latency , 1998 .

[8]  J. Driver,et al.  The neuropsychology of spatial attention , 1998 .

[9]  R. Klein,et al.  A Spatial Gradient of Acceleration and Temporal Extension Underlies Three Illusions of Motion , 1997, Perception.

[10]  O. Hikosaka,et al.  Visual Motion Sensation Yielded by Non-visually Driven Attention , 1997, Vision Research.

[11]  Lew B. Stelmach,et al.  Attentional and Ocular Movements , 1997 .

[12]  J. Mattingley,et al.  Visual extinction and prior entry: Impaired perception of temporal order with intact motion perception after unilateral parietal damage , 1997, Neuropsychologia.

[13]  J. Faubert,et al.  Distinguishing subcortical and cortical influences in visual attention. Subcortical attentional processing. , 1997, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[14]  C. Kennard,et al.  Abnormal temporal dynamics of visual attention in spatial neglect patients , 1997, Nature.

[15]  J. D. Mollon,et al.  Errors of judgement at Greenwich in 1796 , 1996, Nature.

[16]  R. Klein,et al.  Perceptual-motor expectancies interact with covert visual orienting under conditions of endogenous but not exogenous control. , 1994, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.

[17]  O. Hikosaka,et al.  Focal visual attention produces illusory temporal order and motion sensation , 1993, Vision Research.

[18]  L. Stelmach,et al.  Directed attention and perception of temporal order. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[19]  R. D. Frey,et al.  Selective attention, event perception and the criterion of acceptability principle: evidence supporting and rejecting the doctrine of prior entry , 1990 .

[20]  R. Klein,et al.  Is Posner's "beam" the same as Treisman's "glue"?: On the relation between visual orienting and feature integration theory. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[21]  R. Heath Response Time and Temporal Order Judgement in Vision , 1984 .

[22]  P. Cairney Bisensory order judgement and the prior entry hypothesis. , 1975, Acta psychologica.

[23]  P. Bertelson,et al.  The limits of prior entry: Nonsensitivity of temporal order judgments to selective preparation affecting choice reaction time , 1974 .

[24]  R. Sekuler,et al.  The invalidity of “invalid results from the method of constant stimuli”: A common artifact in the methods of psychophysics , 1971 .

[25]  E. S. Robinson A History of Experimental Psychology , 1930 .

[26]  W. B. Pillsbury Lectures or the Elementary Psychology of Feeling and Attention , 1909 .

[27]  M. Posner,et al.  Orienting of Attention* , 1980, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[28]  R. Klein,et al.  Relations among modes of visual orienting , 2000 .

[29]  R A Abrams,et al.  Object-based visual attention with endogenous orienting , 2000, Perception & psychophysics.

[30]  Wiebo Brouwer,et al.  Mental Fatigue after Very Severe Closed Head Injury: Sustained Performance, Mental Effort and Distress at Two Levels of Workload in a Driving Simulator , 1999 .

[31]  H J Müller,et al.  Disinhibition of return: Unnecessary and unlikely , 1998, Perception & psychophysics.

[32]  W C Schmidt,et al.  Inhibition of return is not detected using illusory line motion , 1996, Perception & psychophysics.

[33]  Jon Driver,et al.  Covert Spatial Orienting in Audition: Exogenous and Endogenous Mechanisms , 1994 .

[34]  L. Stelmach,et al.  Attentional modulation of visual processes in motion perception. , 1994 .

[35]  K. Rayner Eye movements and visual cognition : scene perception and reading , 1992 .

[36]  M. Posner,et al.  The attention system of the human brain. , 1990, Annual review of neuroscience.

[37]  E. C. Sanford Attention: Experimental and Critical. , 1897 .

[38]  C. L. M. The Psychology of Attention , 1890, Nature.

[39]  D. Spalding The Principles of Psychology , 1873, Nature.