Synthesized size-sound sound-symbolism

Studies of sound symbolism have shown that people can associate sound and meaning in consistent ways when presented with maximally contrastive stimulus pairs of nonwords such as bouba/kiki (rounded/sharp) or mil/mal (small/big). Recent work has shown the effect extends to antonymic words from natural languages and has proposed a role for shared cross-modal correspondences in biasing form-to-meaning associations. An important open question is how the associations work, and particularly what the role is of sound-symbolic matches versus mismatches. We report on a learning task designed to distinguish between three existing theories by using a spectrum of sound-symbolically matching, mismatching, and neutral (neither matching nor mismatching) stimuli. Synthesized stimuli allow us to control for prosody, and the inclusion of a neutral condition allows a direct test of competing accounts. We find evidence for a sound-symbolic match boost, but not for a mismatch difficulty compared to the neutral condition.

[1]  Simon Kirby,et al.  Phonological and orthographic influences in the bouba–kiki effect , 2017, Psychological research.

[2]  Simon Kirby,et al.  Iconicity and the Emergence of Combinatorial Structure in Language , 2016, Cogn. Sci..

[3]  P. Hagoort,et al.  Sound-symbolism boosts novel word learning. , 2016, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[4]  S. Tufvesson,et al.  What sound symbolism can and cannot do , 2016 .

[5]  Morten H. Christiansen,et al.  Arbitrariness, Iconicity, and Systematicity in Language , 2015, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[6]  G. Lupyan,et al.  Iconicity in English and Spanish and Its Relation to Lexical Category and Age of Acquisition , 2015, PloS one.

[7]  S. Cappa,et al.  When semantics aids phonology: A processing advantage for iconic word forms in aphasia , 2015, Neuropsychologia.

[8]  Mark Dingemanse,et al.  Iconicity in the lab: a review of behavioral, developmental, and neuroimaging research into sound-symbolism , 2015, Front. Psychol..

[9]  J. Tuomainen,et al.  Ideophones in Japanese modulate the P2 and late positive complex responses , 2015, Front. Psychol..

[10]  J. Zlatev Human uniqueness, bodily mimesis and the evolution of language , 2014 .

[11]  K. Emmorey Iconicity impacts linguistic processes : metaphor and anaphora ( a ) Iconic constraints on metaphorical extensions , 2014 .

[12]  G. Vigliocco,et al.  The bridge of iconicity: from a world of experience to the experience of language , 2014, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[13]  S. Kita,et al.  The sound symbolism bootstrapping hypothesis for language acquisition and language evolution , 2014, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[14]  Gary Lupyan,et al.  Meaningless words promote meaningful categorization , 2014, Language and Cognition.

[15]  Julio Santiago,et al.  The Bouba Effect: Sound-Shape Iconicity in Iterated and Implicit Learning , 2014, CogSci.

[16]  S. Kirby,et al.  Synesthesia, Cross-Modality, and Language Evolution , 2013 .

[17]  D. Rendall,et al.  Parsing the role of consonants versus vowels in the classic Takete-Maluma phenomenon. , 2013, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.

[18]  C. Spence,et al.  “Bouba” and “Kiki” in Namibia? A remote culture make similar shape–sound matches, but different shape–taste matches to Westerners , 2013, Cognition.

[19]  Mark Dingemanse,et al.  Advances in the Cross-Linguistic Study of Ideophones , 2012, Lang. Linguistics Compass.

[20]  Alan Nielsen,,et al.  The source and magnitude of sound-symbolic biases in processing artificial word material and their implications for language learning and transmission , 2012, Language and Cognition.

[21]  P. Walker,et al.  The role of sound symbolism in language learning. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[22]  Hanako Yoshida,et al.  A Cross-Linguistic Study of Sound Symbolism in Children's Verb Learning , 2012, Journal of cognition and development : official journal of the Cognitive Development Society.

[23]  Mark E Aveyard Some consonants sound curvy: Effects of sound symbolism on object recognition , 2012, Memory & cognition.

[24]  Patrick D. Thompson,et al.  Sound symbolic naming of novel objects is a graded function , 2011, Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology.

[25]  D. Rendall,et al.  The sound of round: evaluating the sound-symbolic role of consonants in the classic Takete-Maluma phenomenon. , 2011, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.

[26]  Gabriella Vigliocco,et al.  Iconicity as a General Property of Language: Evidence from Spoken and Signed Languages , 2010, Front. Psychology.

[27]  Gert Westermann,et al.  The shape of words in the brain , 2010, Cognition.

[28]  L. Nygaard,et al.  Sound to meaning correspondences facilitate word learning , 2009, Cognition.

[29]  L. Nygaard,et al.  The Semantics of Prosody: Acoustic and Perceptual Evidence of Prosodic Correlates to Word Meaning , 2009, Cogn. Sci..

[30]  S. Kita,et al.  Sound symbolism facilitates early verb learning , 2008, Cognition.

[31]  C. Westbury Implicit sound symbolism in lexical access: Evidence from an interference task , 2005, Brain and Language.

[32]  Thierry Dutoit,et al.  The MBROLA project: towards a set of high quality speech synthesizers free of use for non commercial purposes , 1996, Proceeding of Fourth International Conference on Spoken Language Processing. ICSLP '96.

[33]  W. Köhler Gestalt psychology , 1967 .

[34]  R. Davis The fitness of names to drawings. A cross-cultural study in Tanganyika. , 1961, British journal of psychology.

[35]  E. Sapir A study in phonetic symbolism. , 1929 .