A Mathematical Model for Tamarin's Tool-Use Behavior
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Scott T. Grafton,et al. Forward modeling allows feedback control for fast reaching movements , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[2] D M Wolpert,et al. Multiple paired forward and inverse models for motor control , 1998, Neural Networks.
[3] B. Beck. Animal Tool Behavior: The Use and Manufacture of Tools by Animals , 1980 .
[4] Andrew G. Barto,et al. Reinforcement learning , 1998 .
[5] A. Barto,et al. Adaptive Critics and the Basal Ganglia , 1994 .
[6] Hideko Takeshita,et al. A comparative study of the variety and complexity of object manipulation in captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and bonobos (Pan paniscus) , 1996, Primates.
[7] P. Dayan,et al. Uncertainty-based competition between prefrontal and dorsolateral striatal systems for behavioral control , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.
[8] C. Nelson,et al. The functional emergence of prefrontally-guided working memory systems in four- to eight-year-old children , 1998, Neuropsychologia.
[9] Roland E. Suri,et al. Temporal Difference Model Reproduces Anticipatory Neural Activity , 2001, Neural Computation.
[10] Peter Dayan,et al. A Neural Substrate of Prediction and Reward , 1997, Science.
[11] Jerald D. Kralik,et al. Problem solving and functional design features: experiments on cotton-top tamarins,Saguinus oedipus oedipus , 1999, Animal Behaviour.
[12] S. Kosslyn,et al. Neural foundations of imagery , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[13] Laurie R Santos,et al. Probing the limits of tool competence: Experiments with two non-tool-using species (Cercopithecus aethiops and Saguinus oedipus) , 2006, Animal Cognition.
[14] M. Kawato,et al. A hierarchical neural-network model for control and learning of voluntary movement , 2004, Biological Cybernetics.
[15] T. Matsuzawa. Primate Foundations of Human Intelligence: A View of Tool Use in Nonhuman Primates and Fossil Hominids , 2008 .
[16] Marc D. Hauser,et al. Means-means-end tool choice in cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus): finding the limits on primates’ knowledge of tools , 2005, Animal Cognition.
[17] Rick Grush,et al. The emulation theory of representation: Motor control, imagery, and perception , 2004, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[18] Hiroshi Imamizu,et al. Human cerebellar activity reflecting an acquired internal model of a new tool , 2000, Nature.
[19] G. Hesslow. Conscious thought as simulation of behaviour and perception , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[20] P. Dayan,et al. A framework for mesencephalic dopamine systems based on predictive Hebbian learning , 1996, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[21] T. Torigoe. Comparison of object manipulation among 74 species of non-human primates , 1985, Primates.
[22] J. Fuster. The Prefrontal Cortex , 1997 .
[23] A. Whiten,et al. Cultures in chimpanzees , 1999, Nature.
[24] F A Mussa-Ivaldi,et al. Adaptive representation of dynamics during learning of a motor task , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[25] Mitsuo Kawato,et al. Internal models for motor control and trajectory planning , 1999, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[26] Marc D Hauser,et al. Artifactual kinds and functional design features: what a primate understands without language , 1997, Cognition.