The life-span trajectory of visual perception of 3D objects

[1]  Hermann Bulf,et al.  Visual and proprioceptive feedback differently modulate the spatial representation of number and time in children. , 2017, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[2]  M. Nawrot,et al.  The effects of aging on the perception of depth from motion parallax , 2016, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[3]  A. Norcia,et al.  Development of Three-Dimensional Perception in Human Infants. , 2015, Annual review of vision science.

[4]  Galia Avidan,et al.  Impossible expectations: fMRI adaptation in the lateral occipital complex (LOC) is modulated by the statistical regularities of 3D structural information , 2015, NeuroImage.

[5]  Michael J. Tarr,et al.  Size Precedes View: Developmental Emergence of Invariant Object Representations in Lateral Occipital Complex , 2015, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[6]  Galia Avidan,et al.  The highs and lows of object impossibility: effects of spatial frequency on holistic processing of impossible objects , 2014, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

[7]  David D. Cox,et al.  Do we understand high-level vision? , 2014, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[8]  James T. Townsend,et al.  Methods of Modeling Capacity in Simple Processing Systems , 2014 .

[9]  Galia Avidan,et al.  Holistic processing of impossible objects: Evidence from Garner’s speeded-classification task , 2013, Vision Research.

[10]  G. Andersen Aging and vision: changes in function and performance from optics to perception. , 2012, Wiley interdisciplinary reviews. Cognitive science.

[11]  Albert Yonas,et al.  Infants' sensitivity to pictorial depth cues: a review and meta-analysis of looking studies. , 2012, Infant behavior & development.

[12]  B. Lust,et al.  Early childhood bilingualism leads to advances in executive attention: Dissociating culture and language* , 2011, Bilingualism: Language and Cognition.

[13]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Where cognitive development and aging meet: Face learning ability peaks after age 30 , 2011, Cognition.

[14]  M. Brysbaert,et al.  Combining speed and accuracy in cognitive psychology: Is the inverse efficiency score (IES) a better dependent variable than the mean reaction time (RT) and the percentage of errors (PE)? , 2011 .

[15]  J Farley Norman,et al.  Aging and the perception of slant from optical texture, motion parallax, and binocular disparity , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[16]  Brian Butterworth,et al.  Core information processing deficits in developmental dyscalculia and low numeracy. , 2008, Developmental science.

[17]  Scott P. Johnson,et al.  Development of three-dimensional object completion in infancy. , 2008, Child development.

[18]  J. Farley Norman,et al.  Aging preserves the ability to perceive 3D object shape from static but not deforming boundary contours. , 2008, Acta psychologica.

[19]  V. Goffaux,et al.  Newborns’ face recognition is based on spatial frequencies below 0.5 cycles per degree , 2008, Cognition.

[20]  Scott P. Johnson,et al.  Discrimination of Possible and Impossible Objects in Infancy , 2007, Psychological science.

[21]  Mark H. Johnson Subcortical face processing , 2005, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[22]  Q. Vuong,et al.  The Respective Role of Low and High Spatial Frequencies in Supporting Configural and Featural Processing of Faces , 2005, Perception.

[23]  D. Maurer,et al.  Why 8-year-olds cannot tell the difference between Steve Martin and Paul Newman: factors contributing to the slow development of sensitivity to the spacing of facial features. , 2004, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[24]  I. Kovács Human development of perceptual organization , 2000, Vision Research.

[25]  I. Kovács,et al.  Late maturation of visual spatial integration in humans. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[26]  T. Salthouse The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. , 1996, Psychological review.

[27]  J. Koenderink,et al.  Pictorial surface attitude and local depth comparisons , 1996, Perception & psychophysics.

[28]  Mark H. Johnson,et al.  Biology and Cognitive Development: The Case of Face Recognition , 1993 .

[29]  J. Elman Learning and development in neural networks: the importance of starting small , 1993, Cognition.

[30]  L. Cooper,et al.  Implicit and explicit memory for novel visual objects in older and younger adults. , 1992, Psychology and aging.

[31]  R. Kail Developmental change in speed of processing during childhood and adolescence. , 1991, Psychological bulletin.

[32]  Neil A. Macmillan,et al.  Detection Theory: A User's Guide , 1991 .

[33]  L. Cooper,et al.  Implicit memory for unfamiliar objects depends on access to structural descriptions. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[34]  J. Enns,et al.  Relations between covert orienting and filtering in the development of visual attention. , 1989, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[35]  James T. Enns,et al.  A developmental study of shape integration over space and time. , 1986 .

[36]  G. W. Snedecor STATISTICAL METHODS , 1967 .

[37]  M. Behrmann,et al.  Three‐Dimensional Representations of Objects in Dorsal Cortex are Dissociable from Those in Ventral Cortex , 2017, Cerebral cortex.

[38]  Scott P. Johnson,et al.  Systems in development: motor skill acquisition facilitates three-dimensional object completion. , 2010, Developmental psychology.

[39]  Irene Leo,et al.  How face specialization emerges in the first months of life. , 2007, Progress in brain research.

[40]  James T. Townsend,et al.  The Stochastic Modeling of Elementary Psychological Processes , 1983 .