A spatial oculomotor memory-task performance produces a task-related slow shift in human electroencephalography.

Electroencephalographic (EEG) deflections in humans related to the performance of memory-guided saccades were studied in this work. The EEG deflections were recorded during 2 spatial oculomotor delayed response tasks in which the subject was instructed to make a saccade either to the right or to the left depending on the spatial location of the cue which had been shown in the beginning of the delay period. The EEG deflections were compared to those recorded during a control task in which the subject also made a saccade to the right or to the left after a delay but the requirement to keep spatial information actively in mind was minimized. A slow delay-related shift was recorded during all task conditions. The slow shift was positive in the most frontal and negative in the more posterior recording sites. The negative slow shift in the more posterior recording sites was larger in the memory tasks than in the control task. Since the memory and the control tasks differed mainly in their requirement to hold spatial information in mind it is suggested that the difference in the magnitude of slow shifts between the memory and the control tasks reflects neural activity related to spatial working memory. But although the oculomotor responses in all tasks were similar, the preparatory activities for the impending eye movements may not have been similar and in addition to working memory may have contributed to the observed differences in the slow shifts.

[1]  M E Goldberg,et al.  Participation of prefrontal neurons in the preparation of visually guided eye movements in the rhesus monkey. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.

[2]  B. Larsen,et al.  Cortical activation pattern during saccadic eye movements in humans: Localization by focal cerebral blood flow increases , 1979, Annals of neurology.

[3]  M. Raichle,et al.  The role of cerebral cortex in the generation of voluntary saccades: a positron emission tomographic study. , 1985, Journal of neurophysiology.

[4]  H. Niki,et al.  Prefrontal cortical unit activity and delayed alternation performance in monkeys. , 1971, Journal of neurophysiology.

[5]  J. Grafman,et al.  Distinctions and similarities among working memory processes: an event-related potential study. , 1992, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[6]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Mnemonic coding of visual space in the monkey's dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.

[7]  Masataka Watanabe,et al.  Prefrontal unit activity and delayed response: Relation to cue location versus direction of response , 1976, Brain Research.

[8]  K. Alho,et al.  Intermodal selective attention. I. Effects on event-related potentials to lateralized auditory and visual stimuli. , 1992, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[9]  M. Schlag-Rey,et al.  Evidence for a supplementary eye field. , 1987, Journal of neurophysiology.

[10]  J J Tecce,et al.  Attention Reduction and Suppressed Direct-Current Potentials in the Human Brain , 1969, Science.

[11]  H. Niki Differential activity of prefrontal units during right and left delayed response trials. , 1974, Brain research.

[12]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Visuospatial coding in primate prefrontal neurons revealed by oculomotor paradigms. , 1990, Journal of neurophysiology.

[13]  W. Walter,et al.  Contingent Negative Variation : An Electric Sign of Sensori-Motor Association and Expectancy in the Human Brain , 1964, Nature.

[14]  H Niki,et al.  Prefrontal unit activity during delayed alternation in the monkey. II. Relation to absolute versus relative direction of response. , 1974, Brain research.

[15]  W. P. Dixon,et al.  BMPD statistical software manual , 1988 .

[16]  J. Fuster Unit activity in prefrontal cortex during delayed-response performance: neuronal correlates of transient memory. , 1973, Journal of neurophysiology.

[17]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Dorsolateral prefrontal lesions and oculomotor delayed-response performance: evidence for mnemonic "scotomas" , 1993, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[18]  Diane Kurtzberg,et al.  Topographic analysis of human cortical potentials preceding self-initiated and visually triggered saccades , 1982, Brain Research.

[19]  H. Tanila,et al.  Vertical and horizontal coding of space in the monkey dorsolateral prefrontal cortex , 1990, Brain Research.

[20]  F. Plum Handbook of Physiology. , 1960 .

[21]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Neuronal activity related to saccadic eye movements in the monkey's dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. , 1991, Journal of neurophysiology.

[22]  D L Woods,et al.  Electrophysiologic evidence of increased distractibility after dorsolateral prefrontal lesions , 1986, Neurology.

[23]  G. F. Tremblay,et al.  The Prefrontal Cortex , 1989, Neurology.

[24]  C. Bruce,et al.  Primate frontal eye fields. I. Single neurons discharging before saccades. , 1985, Journal of neurophysiology.

[25]  H Niki,et al.  Prefrontal unit activity during delayed alternation in the monkey. I. Relation to direction of response. , 1974, Brain research.

[26]  K. Kubota,et al.  Visuokinetic activities of primate prefrontal neurons during delayed-response performance. , 1974, Journal of neurophysiology.