The effects of interference on recognition of haptic pictures in blindfolded sighted participants: the modality of representation of haptic information.

Haptic picture recognition was tested in 36 undergraduate students to determine whether haptic representations are visual or multi-modal. Participants explored target haptic pictures and were asked to recognize the target from three alternatives. Each participant completed eight control and eight interference trials in visual, lexical and tactile recognition modalities. For interference, participants were assigned to either an articulatory suppression (repeat "the" at a constant rate) or a visual interference (watch a visual display) condition, to prevent recoding into a verbal or visual code. The results showed that accuracy was higher at control compared to interference trials and for tactile compared to lexical recognition. Additionally, lexical and tactile recognition decreased with articulatory suppression whereas only tactile recognition decreased with visual interference. These findings suggest that haptic pictures may be represented by coordinating visual and verbal codes.

[1]  Linda Pring,et al.  Tactual picture recognition in congenitally blind and sighted children , 1989 .

[2]  J M Kennedy,et al.  Blind children recognizing tactile pictures respond like sighted children given guidance in exploration. , 1998, Scandinavian journal of psychology.

[3]  Rossana de Beni,et al.  Imagery limitations in totally congenitally blind subjects. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[4]  R. Malach,et al.  Early ‘visual’ cortex activation correlates with superior verbal memory performance in the blind , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[5]  A. Paivio Dual coding theory: Retrospect and current status. , 1991 .

[6]  J M Kennedy,et al.  Children's tactual exploration and copying without vision , 2001, International journal of rehabilitation research. Internationale Zeitschrift fur Rehabilitationsforschung. Revue internationale de recherches de readaptation.

[7]  Igor Juricevic,et al.  Haptics and Projection: Drawings by Tracy, a Blind Adult , 2003, Perception.

[8]  Cesare Cornoldi,et al.  Spatial memory and integration processes in congenital blindness , 2004, Neuroreport.

[9]  E. Bushnell,et al.  Children's haptic and cross-modal recognition with familiar and unfamiliar objects. , 1999, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance.

[10]  Andrew T. Woods,et al.  Visual, haptic and crossmodal recognition of scenes , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[11]  Marc H. Bornstein,et al.  The roles of imagery, language, and metamemory in cross-modal transfer in children , 1987, Psychological research.

[12]  J. G. Quinn,et al.  Complexity factors in visuo‐spatial working memory , 2004, Memory.

[13]  John M Kennedy,et al.  Drawings from Gaia, a Blind Girl , 2003, Perception.

[14]  A. D’Angiulli,et al.  Structural equivalences are essential, pictorial conventions are not: Evidence from haptic drawing development in children born completely blind. , 2008 .

[15]  Allan Paivio,et al.  Spatial and verbal abilities in children's crossmodal recognition: A dual coding approach. , 1989 .

[16]  C. Miles,et al.  Tactile short-term memory revisited. , 1996, Memory.

[17]  R. Klatzky,et al.  Visual mediation and the haptic recognition of two-dimensional pictures of common objects , 1990, Perception & psychophysics.

[18]  J. Kennedy A psychology of picture perception , 1974 .

[19]  J G Quinn,et al.  Irrelevant Pictures in Visual Working Memory , 1996, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[20]  J. Kennedy,et al.  Foreshortening, Convergence and Drawings from a Blind Adult , 2006, Perception.

[21]  D. Levine,et al.  Visual and spatial mental imagery: Dissociable systems of representation , 1988, Cognitive Psychology.

[22]  A D Baddeley,et al.  Tactile Short-Term Memory , 1969, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[23]  A. D’Angiulli,et al.  Enhanced tactile encoding and memory recognition in congenital blindness , 2002, International journal of rehabilitation research. Internationale Zeitschrift fur Rehabilitationsforschung. Revue internationale de recherches de readaptation.

[24]  M. D’Esposito Working memory. , 2008, Handbook of clinical neurology.

[25]  R. Simes,et al.  An improved Bonferroni procedure for multiple tests of significance , 1986 .

[26]  M A Heller,et al.  Tactual picture identification by blind and sighted people: Effects of providing categorical information , 1996, Perception & psychophysics.

[27]  Marie-Chantal Wanet-Defalque,et al.  Abilities and strategies of blind and sighted subjects in visuo-spatial imagery. , 2004, Acta psychologica.

[28]  S. Lacey,et al.  Mental representation in visual/haptic crossmodal memory: evidence from interference effects , 2006, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[29]  John M Kennedy,et al.  Haptic Pictures: Fit Judgments Predict Identification, Recognition Memory, and Confidence , 2002, Perception.

[30]  M. Heller Picture and Pattern Perception in the Sighted and the Blind: The Advantage of the Late Blind , 1989, Perception.

[31]  A. Paivio Coding Distinctions and Repetition Effects in Memory1 , 1975 .

[32]  P. Pietrini,et al.  Imagery and spatial processes in blindness and visual impairment , 2008, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[33]  Amedeo D'Angiulli,et al.  Raised-Line Pictures, Blindness, and Tactile “Beliefs”: An Observational Case Study , 2007 .

[34]  J. G. Snodgrass,et al.  A standardized set of 260 pictures: norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. Human learning and memory.

[35]  Christopher Miles,et al.  Recognition memory for tactile sequences , 2002, Memory.