Attention and Sensory Gain Control: A Peripheral Visual Process?

Attention-related sensory gain control in human extra-striate cortex is believed to improve the acuity of visual perception. Yet given wide variance in the spatial resolution of vision across the retina, it remains unclear whether sensory gain operates homogenously between foveal and nonfoveal retinotopic locations. To address this issue, we used event-related potentials (ERPs) in a variant of the canonical spatial attention task. Participants were cued to expect targets at either fixation (foveal targets) or at a location several degrees above fixation (parafoveal targets). At both target locations, manual reaction times were shorter for cued relative to uncued targets, indicating that attention was consistently oriented to the cued location. Nevertheless, attention-related increases in sensory-evoked cortical activity were only observed at the parafoveal target location, as measured by the amplitude of the lateral occipital P1 ERP component. A second experiment replicated this data pattern using targets with lower stimulus contrast, indicating that the absence of a P1 effect for foveal targets could not be attributed to a saturated P1 response under higher-contrast stimulus conditions. When considered in light of retinogeniculate projections to cortex showing systematic changes in their physiological organization beginning within a degree of visual angle of the fovea, our findings support the proposal that the strategic functions of visual attention may vary with the retinotopic location involved.

[1]  Marisa Carrasco,et al.  Speed of visual processing increases with eccentricity , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[2]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Organization of visual inputs to the inferior temporal and posterior parietal cortex in macaques , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[3]  S. Zeki A vision of the brain , 1993 .

[4]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Mechanisms of directed attention in the human extrastriate cortex as revealed by functional MRI. , 1998, Science.

[5]  G. Mangun Neural mechanisms of visual selective attention. , 1995, Psychophysiology.

[6]  M. Eimer “Sensory gating” as a mechanism for visuospatial orienting: Electrophysiological evidence from trial-by-trial cuing experiments , 1994, Perception & psychophysics.

[7]  Helen J. Neville,et al.  Attention to central and peripheral visual space in a movement detection task: an event-related potential and behavioral study. I. Normal hearing adults , 1987, Brain Research.

[8]  G R Mangun,et al.  Attention and spatial selection: Electrophysiological evidence for modulation by perceptual load , 2000, Perception & psychophysics.

[9]  Carlo Miniussi,et al.  Watching where you look: modulation of visual processing of foveal stimuli by spatial attention , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[10]  Yan Bao,et al.  Two spatially separated attention systems in the visual field: evidence from inhibition of return , 2007, Cognitive Processing.

[11]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Visual evoked potentials and selective attention to points in space , 1977 .

[12]  Robert Shapley,et al.  Parallel neural pathways and visual function , 1994 .

[13]  E. Vogel,et al.  Sensory gain control (amplification) as a mechanism of selective attention: electrophysiological and neuroimaging evidence. , 1998, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[14]  J. J. Moyle,et al.  Event-related potentials elicited during a visual Go-Nogo task in adults with phenylketonuria , 2006, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[15]  Marisa Carrasco,et al.  Attention improves or impairs visual performance by enhancing spatial resolution , 1998, Nature.

[16]  G. Mangun,et al.  Perceptual Load and Visuocortical Processing: Event-Related Potentials Reveal Sensory-Level Selection , 2001, Psychological science.

[17]  R. Desimone,et al.  Attention Increases Sensitivity of V4 Neurons , 2000, Neuron.

[18]  J. Bullier,et al.  Anatomical segregation of two cortical visual pathways in the macaque monkey , 1990, Visual Neuroscience.

[19]  M. Rugg,et al.  Electrophysiology of Mind: Event-Related Brain Potentials and Cognition , 1995 .

[20]  R. Desimone,et al.  Attention Increases Sensitivity of V4 Neurons , 2000, Neuron.

[21]  David J. Turk,et al.  Placing a tool in the spotlight: spatial attention modulates visuomotor responses in cortex , 2005, NeuroImage.

[22]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Modulations of sensory-evoked brain potentials indicate changes in perceptual processing during visual-spatial priming. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[23]  Todd C. Handy,et al.  Event-related potentials : a methods handbook , 2005 .

[24]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Graspable objects grab attention when the potential for action is recognized , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[25]  M. Gazzaniga,et al.  Combined spatial and temporal imaging of brain activity during visual selective attention in humans , 1994, Nature.

[26]  P. Fox,et al.  Retinotopic organization of early visual spatial attention effects as revealed by PET and ERPs , 1997, Human brain mapping.

[27]  Steven A. Hillyard,et al.  Effects of spatial cuing on luminance detectability: Psychophysical and electrophysiological evidence for early selection. , 1994 .

[28]  S. Luck,et al.  Electrocortical substrates of visual selective attention , 1993 .

[29]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Identification of early visual evoked potential generators by retinotopic and topographic analyses , 1994 .

[30]  DH Hubel,et al.  Psychophysical evidence for separate channels for the perception of form, color, movement, and depth , 1987, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[31]  F. Bloom Principles of Neural Science, 3rd ed , 1993 .

[32]  M. Posner,et al.  Orienting of Attention* , 1980, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[33]  J. J. Lange,et al.  An ERP study of visual spatial attention and letter target detection for isoluminant and nonisoluminant stimuli. , 1997, Psychophysiology.

[34]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  The neural basis of biased competition in human visual cortex , 2001, Neuropsychologia.

[35]  H. Neville,et al.  Attention to central and peripheral visual space in a movement detection task. III. Separate effects of auditory deprivation and acquisition of a visual language , 1987, Brain Research.

[36]  E. Bizzi,et al.  The Cognitive Neurosciences , 1996 .

[37]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Reorienting attention across the horizontal and vertical meridians: Evidence in favor of a premotor theory of attention , 1987, Neuropsychologia.

[38]  Marlene C. Richter,et al.  Retinotopic Organization and Functional Subdivisions of the Human Lateral Geniculate Nucleus: A High-Resolution Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[39]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Allocation of visual attention to spatial locations: Tradeoff functions for event-related brain potentials and detection performance , 1990, Perception & psychophysics.

[40]  Paul Azzopardi,et al.  Uneven mapping of magnocellular and parvocellular projections from the lateral geniculate nucleus to the striate cortex in the macaque monkey , 1999, Vision Research.

[41]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. , 1995, Annual review of neuroscience.

[42]  J. Duncan,et al.  Competitive brain activity in visual attention , 1997, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[43]  M. Goodale,et al.  The visual brain in action , 1995 .

[44]  N. Drasdo,et al.  Receptive field densities of the ganglion cells of the human retina , 1989, Vision Research.

[45]  M Eimer,et al.  Spatial cueing, sensory gating and selective response preparation: an ERP study on visuo-spatial orienting. , 1993, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[46]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Modulations of sensory-evoked brain potentials indicate changes in perceptual processing during visual-spatial priming. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[47]  Steven A. Hillyard,et al.  Mechanisms and models of se-lective attention , 1995 .

[48]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Involvement of striate and extrastriate visual cortical areas in spatial attention , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[49]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Space and selective attention , 1994 .

[50]  D. Dacey The mosaic of midget ganglion cells in the human retina , 1993, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[51]  B. Boycott,et al.  Cortical magnification factor and the ganglion cell density of the primate retina , 1989, Nature.

[52]  Helen J. Neville,et al.  Attention to central and peripheral visual space in a movement detection task: an event-related potential and behavioral study. II. Congenitally deaf adults , 1987, Brain Research.

[53]  E. DeYoe,et al.  Concurrent processing in the primate visual cortex. , 1995 .