Predictive feedback control and Fitts–law

Fitts–law is a well established empirical formula, known for encapsulating the ‘speed-accuracy trade-off’ For discrete, manual movements from a starting location to a target, Fitts–law relates movement duration to the distance moved and target size. The widespread empirical success of the formula is suggestive of underlying principles of human movement control. There have been previous attempts to relate Fitts–law to engineering-type control hypotheses and it has been shown that the law is exactly consistent with the closed-loop step-response of a time-delayed, first-order system. Assuming only the operation of closed-loop feedback, either continuous or intermittent, this paper asks whether such feedback should be predictive or not predictive to be consistent with Fitts law. Since Fitts–law is equivalent to a time delay separated from a first-order system, known control theory implies that the controller must be predictive. A predictive controller moves the time-delay outside the feedback loop such that the closed-loop response can be separated into a time delay and rational function whereas a non- predictive controller retains a state delay within feedback loop which is not consistent with Fitts–law. Using sufficient parameters, a high-order non-predictive controller could approximately reproduce Fitts–law. However, such high-order, ‘non-parametric’controllers are essentially empirical in nature, without physical meaning, and therefore are conceptually inferior to the predictive controller. It is a new insight that using closed-loop feedback, prediction is required to physically explain Fitts–law. The implication is that prediction is an inherent part of the ‘speed-accuracy trade-off’

[1]  Peter J. Gawthrop,et al.  Predictive pole-placement control with linear models , 2002, Autom..

[2]  Joseph A. Doeringer,et al.  Intermittency in preplanned elbow movements persists in the absence of visual feedback. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[3]  Peter J. Gawthrop,et al.  Intermittent Predictive Control of an Inverted Pendulum , 2006 .

[4]  P D Neilson,et al.  Influence of intermittency and synergy on grasping. , 1999, Motor control.

[5]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Internal models in the cerebellum , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[6]  W. Steinway Estimation theory with applications to communication and control: Andrew P. Sage and James L. Melsa. 529 pages, diagrams, 6×9 in. New York, McGraw-Hill, 1971. Price $16.50. , 1972 .

[7]  Ian David Loram,et al.  The frequency of human, manual adjustments in balancing an inverted pendulum is constrained by intrinsic physiological factors , 2006, The Journal of physiology.

[8]  D. W. Repperger,et al.  Why engineers should know and use Fitts' law , 1997, Proceedings of the 19th Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society. 'Magnificent Milestones and Emerging Opportunities in Medical Engineering' (Cat. No.97CH36136).

[9]  David J. Cannon Experiments with a target-threshold control theory model for deriving Fitts' law parameters for human-machine systems , 1994 .

[10]  Peter J. Gawthrop Intermittent constrained predictive control of mechanical systems , 2004 .

[11]  Reza Shadmehr,et al.  Computational nature of human adaptive control during learning of reaching movements in force fields , 1999, Biological Cybernetics.

[12]  J. G. Hollands,et al.  Engineering Psychology and Human Performance , 1984 .

[13]  R. Miall,et al.  Adaptation to visual feedback delays in manual tracking: evidence against the Smith Predictor model of human visually guided action , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[14]  David E. Meyer,et al.  Speed—Accuracy Tradeoffs in Aimed Movements: Toward a Theory of Rapid Voluntary Action , 2018, Attention and Performance XIII.

[15]  Peter J. Gawthrop,et al.  Emulator-based control and internal model control: Complementary approaches to robust control design , 1996, Autom..

[16]  P. Fitts The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. , 1954, Journal of experimental psychology.

[17]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Is the cerebellum a smith predictor? , 1993, Journal of motor behavior.

[18]  John M. Flach,et al.  Control Theory for Humans: Quantitative Approaches To Modeling Performance , 2002 .

[19]  Katsuhisa Furuta,et al.  Internal model and saturating actuation in human operation from view of human-adaptive mechatronics , 2005, IEEE Transactions on Industrial Electronics.

[20]  Peter J. Gawthrop,et al.  Estimation and control of mechatronic systems using sensitivity bond graphs , 2000 .

[21]  P. D. Neilson,et al.  Internal models and intermittency: A theoretical account of human tracking behavior , 2004, Biological Cybernetics.

[22]  Daniel M. Wolpert,et al.  Making smooth moves , 2022 .

[23]  K. J. W. Craik Theory of the human operator in control systems; the operator as an engineering system. , 1947 .

[24]  R Plamondon,et al.  Speed/accuracy trade-offs in target-directed movements , 1997, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[25]  Ian David Loram,et al.  Human balancing of an inverted pendulum with a compliant linkage: neural control by anticipatory intermittent bias , 2003, The Journal of physiology.

[26]  Peter J. Gawthrop,et al.  Open-loop intermittent feedback control: practical continuous-time GPC , 1999 .

[27]  P. R. Davidson,et al.  Widespread access to predictive models in the motor system: a short review , 2005, Journal of neural engineering.

[28]  R. E. Kalman,et al.  When Is a Linear Control System Optimal , 1964 .

[29]  Daniel M Wolpert,et al.  Computational principles of sensorimotor control that minimize uncertainty and variability , 2007, The Journal of physiology.

[30]  M. D. Neilson,et al.  An overview of adaptive model theory: solving the problems of redundancy, resources, and nonlinear interactions in human movement control , 2005, Journal of neural engineering.

[31]  Edward M. Connelly,et al.  A Control Model: An Alternative Interpretation of Fitts' Law , 1984 .

[32]  R. Miall,et al.  Intermittency in human manual tracking tasks. , 1993, Journal of motor behavior.

[33]  K. J. Craik THEORY OF THE HUMAN OPERATOR IN CONTROL SYSTEMS , 1948 .

[34]  Duane T. McRuer,et al.  Human dynamics in man-machine systems , 1980, Autom..

[35]  I. Scott MacKenzie,et al.  Towards a standard for pointing device evaluation, perspectives on 27 years of Fitts' law research in HCI , 2004, Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud..

[36]  Huibert Kwakernaak,et al.  Linear Optimal Control Systems , 1972 .

[37]  J. Gibson,et al.  Analytical design of linear feedback controls , 1958 .

[38]  O. J. M. Smith,et al.  A controller to overcome dead time , 1959 .

[39]  Reza Shadmehr,et al.  Evidence for a Forward Dynamics Model in Human Adaptive Motor Control , 1998, NIPS.

[40]  Peter J. Gawthrop Intermittent predictive control , 2002 .

[41]  Daniel M. Wolpert,et al.  Forward Models for Physiological Motor Control , 1996, Neural Networks.

[42]  Herman van der Kooij,et al.  A multisensory integration model of human stance control , 1999, Biological Cybernetics.

[43]  W D Beggs,et al.  The accuracy of aiming at a target. Some further evidence for a theory of intermittent control. , 1972, Acta psychologica.

[44]  J. E. Marshall,et al.  Control of Time-Delay Systems , 1981, IEEE Transactions on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics.

[45]  David L. Kleinman,et al.  Optimal control of linear systems with time-delay and observation noise , 1969 .

[46]  Martin Lakie,et al.  Manually controlled human balancing using visual, vestibular and proprioceptive senses involves a common, low frequency neural process , 2006, The Journal of physiology.

[47]  O. Jacobs,et al.  Introduction to Control Theory , 1976, IEEE Transactions on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics.