Ethnocultural influences on variation in chronic pain perception

&NA; In multiple studies cultural affiliation has been found to have an important influence on perception of and response to experimental and acute pain. Despite that evidence little work has been directed to understanding the cultural dimensions of the chronic pain experience. We present the results of a quantitative study of reported chronic pain perception in 372 chronic pain patients in six ethnic groups, who were under treatment at a multidisciplinary pain‐management center. The role of ethnic and cultural experiences in the complex array of physical, cultural, psychological and social factors which influence the chronic pain experience is identified. Ethnocultural affiliation is important to chronic pain perception and response variation. In this study population the best predictors of pain intensity variation are ethnic group affiliation and locus of control (LOC) style (ethnic group identity is also a predictor of LOC style). It appears that pain intensity variation may be affected by differences in attitudes, beliefs and emotional and psychological states associated with the different ethnic groups. This study suggests a biocultural model may be useful in conceptualizing the complex interaction of biological, cultural and psychosocial factors in the process of human pain perception. Although it is likely that intense pain affects attitudes and emotions, it is also very likely that attitudes and emotions influence reported perceptions of pain intensity. Pain intensity variation in this study population is not significantly associated with diagnosis, present medication types, or types of past treatments or surgeries for pain.

[1]  C. Sargent Between death and shame: dimensions of pain in Bariba culture. , 1984, Social science & medicine.

[2]  W. Torgerson,et al.  On the Language of Pain , 1971, Anesthesiology.

[3]  R. Sternbach Pain Patients: Traits and Treatments , 1974 .

[4]  K. Craig,et al.  Autonomic correlates of pain thresholds influenced by social modeling. , 1974, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[5]  L. Festinger A Theory of Social Comparison Processes , 1954 .

[6]  M. Weisenberg Pain and pain control. , 1977, Psychological bulletin.

[7]  R. Melzack The McGill Pain Questionnaire: Major properties and scoring methods , 1975, PAIN.

[8]  K. Shum,et al.  Response to cold pressor pain and to acupuncture analgesia in oriental and occidental subjects , 1977, Pain.

[9]  D. Moore The management of pain. , 1954, The Illinois medical journal.

[10]  J Fifield,et al.  Appraisals of control and predictability in adapting to a chronic disease. , 1987, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[11]  M. Zborowski People in Pain , 1969 .

[12]  M. Cousins International association for the study of pain , 1989, Pain.

[13]  P. Wall,et al.  Pain mechanisms: a new theory. , 1965, Science.

[14]  N. Krause,et al.  Stress and well-being: the buffering role of locus of control beliefs. , 1984, Social science & medicine.

[15]  J. Rotter Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforcement. , 1966, Psychological monographs.

[16]  H. Flor,et al.  Pain and families. I. Etiology, maintenance, and psychosocial impact , 1987, Pain.

[17]  E. Spierings,et al.  Endogenous pain control mechanisms , 1979, Annals of neurology.

[18]  R. Tait,et al.  A comparison of health locus of control beliefs in low-back patients from the U.S. and New Zealand , 1982, Pain.

[19]  J. Marbach,et al.  Ethnicity and the pain experience. , 1984, Social science & medicine.

[20]  P. Wall Pain. A psychophysiological analysis , 1972 .

[21]  H. Merskey Pain terms: a list with definitions and notes on usage. Recommended by the IASP Subcommittee on Taxonomy. , 1979, Pain.

[22]  S. Linton,et al.  Controlling Pain Reports through Operant Conditioning: A Laboratory Demonstration , 1985, Perceptual and motor skills.

[23]  K. Geisinger,et al.  The language of low back pain: Factor structure of the McGill pain questionnaire , 1980, PAIN.

[24]  J. Streltzer,et al.  The Influence of Cultural Group on the Undertreatment of Postoperative Pain1 , 1981, Psychosomatic medicine.

[25]  Lloyd K. Daniels,et al.  Behavioral Methods for Chronic Pain and Illness , 1978 .

[26]  Pain tolerance and group identification. , 1967, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[27]  P. Salovey,et al.  The McGill pain questionnaire reconsidered: Confirming the factor structure and examining appropriate uses , 1985, Pain.

[28]  E. Shoben,et al.  An empirical study of the etiology of dental fears. , 1954, Journal of clinical psychology.

[29]  Bates Ms,et al.  Ethnic variations in the chronic pain experience. , 1992 .

[30]  I. K. Zola Culture and symptoms--an analysis of patients' presenting complaints. , 1966, American sociological review.

[31]  W. Edwards,et al.  Ethnic variations in the chronic pain experience. , 1992, Ethnicity & disease.

[32]  G. Burrows,et al.  CLINICAL MEASUREMENT OF PAIN , 1979, The Medical journal of Australia.

[33]  R. T. Garza,et al.  The validity of locus of control dimensions for Chicano populations. , 1977, Journal of personality assessment.

[34]  M. S. Bates Ethnicity and pain: a biocultural model. , 1987, Social science & medicine.

[35]  D. Ciccone,et al.  Cognitive dimensions of chronic pain. , 1984, Social science & medicine.

[36]  H. M. Dekking,et al.  On adaptation. , 1945, Ophthalmologica. Journal international d'ophtalmologie. International journal of ophthalmology. Zeitschrift fur Augenheilkunde.

[37]  M Agathon,et al.  [Behavior therapy]. , 1978, Psychotherapy and psychosomatics.

[38]  L. Kenner,et al.  Ethnic and Sex Differences in Response to Clinical and Induced Pain in Chronic Spinal Pain Patients , 1984, Spine.