Learning directions of objects specified by vision, spatial audition, or auditory spatial language.

The modality by which object azimuths (directions) are presented affects learning of multiple locations. In Experiment 1, participants learned sets of three and five object azimuths specified by a visual virtual environment, spatial audition (3D sound), or auditory spatial language. Five azimuths were learned faster when specified by spatial modalities (vision, audition) than by language. Experiment 2 equated the modalities for proprioceptive cues and eliminated spatial cues unique to vision (optic flow) and audition (differential binaural signals). There remained a learning disadvantage for spatial language. We attribute this result to the cost of indirect processing from words to spatial representations.

[1]  M. Potter,et al.  Time to understand pictures and words , 1975, Nature.

[2]  M. Battacchi,et al.  Memory processing of spatial order as transmitted by auditory information in the absence of visual cues , 1981 .

[3]  Leslie G. Ungerleider Two cortical visual systems , 1982 .

[4]  Elizabeth M. Zelinski,et al.  Memory for spatial information in young and old adults. , 1983 .

[5]  Kathy Pezdek,et al.  American Psychological Association, Inc. Memory for Items and Their Spatial Locations by Young and Elderly Adults , 2022 .

[6]  P L Brennan,et al.  Cognitive mapping and elderly adults: verbal and location memory for urban landmarks. , 1984, Journal of gerontology.

[7]  Kathy Pezdek,et al.  Spatial Memory for Objects and Words , 1986 .

[8]  M Naveh-Benjamin,et al.  Coding of spatial location information: an automatic process? , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[9]  John G. Seamon,et al.  Double dissociation of spatial and object visual memory: Evidence from selective interference in intact human subjects , 1993, Neuropsychologia.

[10]  I. Whishaw,et al.  Fundamentals of Human Neuropsychology , 1995 .

[11]  G. Musen Effects of Task Demands on Implicit Memory for Object-Location Associations , 1996 .

[12]  E. D. de Haan,et al.  What Was Where? Memory for Object Locations , 1996, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[13]  D A Allport,et al.  Independent coding of target distance and direction in visuo-spatial working memory , 1997, Psychological research.

[14]  J. Rauschecker Cortical processing of complex sounds , 1998, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[15]  R. Klatzky,et al.  Assessing auditory distance perception using perceptually directed action , 1998, Perception & psychophysics.

[16]  J. Rauschecker Parallel Processing in the Auditory Cortex of Primates , 1998, Audiology and Neurotology.

[17]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Rehearsal in Spatial Working Memory: Evidence From Neuroimaging , 1999 .

[18]  Kathleen C. Kirasic,et al.  Age differences in adults' spatial abilities, learning environmental layout, and wayfinding behavior , 2000, Spatial Cogn. Comput..

[19]  Glyn W. Humphreys,et al.  The neural substrates of action retrieval: An examination of semantic and visual routes to action , 2002 .

[20]  Jörg Lewald,et al.  Visual and proprioceptive shifts in perceived egocentric direction induced by eye-position , 2000, Vision Research.

[21]  A. Friederici,et al.  Slow cortical potentials during retention of object, spatial, and verbal information. , 2001, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[22]  Matthew Brett,et al.  Large, colourful or noisy? Attribute- and modality-specific activations during retrieval of perceptual attribute knowledge , 2001, NeuroImage.

[23]  Roberta L Klatzky,et al.  Spatial updating of locations specified by 3-d sound and spatial language. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.