Approaching and departing bees learn different cues to the distance of a landmark

Bees learn both the absolute distance and the apparent size of landmarks in the vicinity of a foraging site. They learn about landmarks both when approaching and when leaving the site. Whereas learning on arrival can take place on every visit to the food source, learning on departure is limited to the first few visits, when the bee Turns Back and Looks (TBL) at the feeder in a stereotyped manoeuvre before flying off. We investigated whether one specific function of TBLs is to acquire information about the absolute distance of landmarks from the feeding site. Bees were trained to forage from a feeder which lay at a fixed distance from a cylinder. During training, bees were exposed to the cylinder either only while they approached and landed on the feeder, or only on their departure from it, or at both of these times. Tests on trained bees immediately after the TBL phase revealed that those bees which had viewed the cylinder only on arrival had learnt the apparent size of the cylinder, but not its distance from the feeder. In contrast, bees which saw the cylinder on departure had learnt its absolute distance. They also learnt the cylinder's apparent size, provided that the cylinder was close to the feeder. Bees which had viewed the cylinder on arrival as well as on departure learnt both absolute distance and apparent size. Distance dominated the bees' behaviour in the initial phase of learning, apparent size was more important later on. We suggest that early during learning bees need information about the 3-D structure of the environment so that they can identify those landmarks close to a foraging site which will specify accurately the site's position. This information is acquired during TBLs. Later, landmark guidance can be achieved by 2-D image matching.

[1]  B. A. Cartwright,et al.  Short Communications: How Honey-Bees Know their Distance from a Near-By Visual Landmark , 1979 .

[2]  R. Wehner Spatial Vision in Arthropods , 1981 .

[3]  M. Srinivasan,et al.  Motion cues provide the bee's visual world with a third dimension , 1988, Nature.

[4]  M. Srinivasan,et al.  Range perception through apparent image speed in freely flying honeybees , 1991, Visual Neuroscience.

[5]  M. Bitterman,et al.  Learning by honeybees (Apis mellifera) on arrival at and departure from a feeding place. , 1991, Journal of comparative psychology.

[6]  T. Collett,et al.  Looking and learning: a spatial pattern in the orientation flight of the wasp Vespula vulgaris , 1993, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[7]  R. Menzel Das Gedächtnis der Honigbiene für Spektralfarben , 1968, Zeitschrift für vergleichende Physiologie.

[8]  T. Collett,et al.  The use of visual landmarks by honeybees: Bees weight landmarks according to their distance from the goal , 1987, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[9]  M. Lehrer Why do bees turn back and look? , 1993, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[10]  Elisabeth Opfinger Über die Orientierung der Biene an der Futterquelle , 1931, Zeitschrift für vergleichende Physiologie.

[11]  J. Zeil Orientation flights of solitary wasps (Cerceris; Sphecidae; Hymenoptera) , 1993, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[12]  J. Zeil Orientation flights of solitary wasps (Cerceris; Sphecidae; Hymenoptera) , 1993, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[13]  T. S. Collett,et al.  Landmark learning in bees , 1983, Journal of comparative physiology.

[14]  J. L. Gould Timing of landmark learning by honey bees , 1988, Journal of Insect Behavior.

[15]  R. Wehner,et al.  Visual spatial memory in desert ants,Cataglyphis bicolor (Hymenoptera: Formicidae) , 1979, Experientia.

[16]  M. Bitterman,et al.  Landmark learning by honeybees , 2005, Journal of Insect Behavior.

[17]  M. Lehrer,et al.  Bees which turn back and look , 1991, Naturwissenschaften.