Reply: Replicability and impact of statistics in the detection of neural responses of consciousness.

Sir, We read with interest the letter by Gabriel and colleagues (2016) addressing the major issue of replicability when probing conscious processing in non-communicating patients. This question—as well as the choice of the optimal statistical methodology—concerns the whole field of functional brain imaging in cognitive neuroscience (Kriegeskorte et al. , 2009), but its importance obviously culminates in single-subject analyses of non-communicating patients (see for instance the recent debate in Cruse et al. , 2011, 2013; Goldfine et al. , 2012). Gabriel et al. reacted to a recent discussion (Naccache et al. , 2015; Tzovara et al. , 2015 a , b ) following a report by Tzovara et al. (2015 a ), who adapted our auditory ‘local-global’ bedside EEG test (Bekinschtein et al. , 2009) to test comatose patients. Briefly, in the local-global paradigm two levels of regularities are manipulated: local auditory irregularities correspond to a change of sound within a trial, whereas global irregularities correspond to a change of sound sequence across trials. When analysing data according to the local irregularities, one can typically extract a mismatch negativity response observable even in unconscious states. In sharp contrast, when analysing event-related potentials (ERPs) to violations of global irregularities, we previously showed that a late global effect was present only in conscious or minimally conscious patients (Bekinschtein et al. , 2009; Faugeras et al. , 2011, 2012). Two problems emerged from the study of Tzovara et al. (2015 a ), first, this ERP global effect was found positive in the vast majority of conscious controls we tested at two distinct sites using high-density EEG: 18/18 (100%) in Paris, France (with 256 electrodes), and 7 to 10/10 (70 to 100%) with the monaural and binaural versions of the task, respectively in Cambridge, UK …

[1]  L. Naccache,et al.  Survival and consciousness recovery are better in the minimally conscious state than in the vegetative state , 2018, Brain injury.

[2]  Steven Laureys,et al.  “Look at my classifier's result”: Disentangling unresponsive from (minimally) conscious patients , 2017, NeuroImage.

[3]  T. Moulin,et al.  Replicability and impact of statistics in the detection of neural responses of consciousness. , 2016, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[4]  Athina Tzovara,et al.  Prediction of awakening from hypothermic postanoxic coma based on auditory discrimination , 2016, Annals of neurology.

[5]  M. Brandon Westover,et al.  Quantification of EEG reactivity in comatose patients , 2016, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[6]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Neural detection of complex sound sequences or of statistical regularities in the absence of consciousness? , 2015, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[7]  P. Kaplan,et al.  EEG as an Indicator of Cerebral Functioning in Postanoxic Coma. , 2015, Journal of clinical neurophysiology : official publication of the American Electroencephalographic Society.

[8]  Michael C. Frank,et al.  Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science , 2015, Science.

[9]  M. D. Lucia,et al.  Decoding auditory EEG responses in healthy and clinical populations: A comparative study , 2015, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[10]  A. Tzovara,et al.  Neural detection of complex sound sequences in the absence of consciousness. , 2015, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[11]  Steven Laureys,et al.  Cognitive auditory evoked potentials in coma: can you hear me? , 2015, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[12]  C. Klein Reliability in Cognitive Neuroscience: A Meta-Meta Analysis , 2015 .

[13]  Jean-Rémi King,et al.  Probing ERP correlates of verbal semantic processing in patients with impaired consciousness , 2015, Neuropsychologia.

[14]  Steven Laureys,et al.  Diagnostic precision of PET imaging and functional MRI in disorders of consciousness: a clinical validation study , 2014, The Lancet.

[15]  M. Murray,et al.  Automated Auditory Mismatch Negativity Paradigm Improves Coma Prognostic Accuracy After Cardiac Arrest and Therapeutic Hypothermia , 2014, Journal of clinical neurophysiology.

[16]  Dominique Morlet,et al.  MMN and Novelty P3 in Coma and Other Altered States of Consciousness: A Review , 2013, Brain Topography.

[17]  M. Sigman,et al.  Large scale screening of neural signatures of consciousness in patients in a vegetative or minimally conscious state. , 2014, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[18]  Athina Tzovara,et al.  Robust discrimination between EEG responses to categories of environmental sounds in early coma , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[19]  A. Luxen,et al.  Automated Analysis of Background EEG and Reactivity During Therapeutic Hypothermia in Comatose Patients After Cardiac Arrest , 2014, Clinical EEG and neuroscience.

[20]  J. R. King,et al.  Single-trial decoding of auditory novelty responses facilitates the detection of residual consciousness , 2013, NeuroImage.

[21]  G. Tononi,et al.  A Theoretically Based Index of Consciousness Independent of Sensory Processing and Behavior , 2013, Science Translational Medicine.

[22]  Michel J. A. M. van Putten,et al.  Quantification of the adult EEG background pattern , 2013, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[23]  Quentin Noirhomme,et al.  Reanalysis of “Bedside detection of awareness in the vegetative state: a cohort study” , 2013, The Lancet.

[24]  Srivas Chennu,et al.  Reanalysis of “Bedside detection of awareness in the vegetative state: a cohort study” – Authors' reply , 2013, The Lancet.

[25]  Steven Laureys,et al.  Diffusion Tensor Imaging to Predict Long-term Outcome after Cardiac Arrest: A Bicentric Pilot Study , 2012, Anesthesiology.

[26]  M. Murray,et al.  A Tutorial Review of Electrical Neuroimaging From Group-Average to Single-Trial Event-Related Potentials , 2012, Developmental neuropsychology.

[27]  Xiaofeng Jia,et al.  Hypothermia Amplifies Somatosensory-evoked Potentials in Uninjured Rats , 2012, Journal of neurosurgical anesthesiology.

[28]  Christoph M. Michel,et al.  Decoding stimulus-related information from single-trial EEG responses based on voltage topographies , 2012, Pattern Recognit..

[29]  Mary M. Conte,et al.  Bedside detection of awareness in the vegetative state , 2012, The Lancet.

[30]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Event related potentials elicited by violations of auditory regularities in patients with impaired consciousness , 2012, Neuropsychologia.

[31]  Srivas Chennu,et al.  Bedside detection of awareness in the vegetative state: a cohort study , 2011, The Lancet.

[32]  N. Schiff,et al.  Arterial spin labeling and altered cerebral blood flow patterns in the minimally conscious state , 2011, Neurology.

[33]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Probing consciousness with event-related potentials in the vegetative state , 2011, Neurology.

[34]  Stefan Haufe,et al.  Single-trial analysis and classification of ERP components — A tutorial , 2011, NeuroImage.

[35]  T Sarraf,et al.  Long-term outcomes of chronic minimally conscious and vegetative states , 2010, Neurology.

[36]  W. K. Simmons,et al.  Circular analysis in systems neuroscience: the dangers of double dipping , 2009, Nature Neuroscience.

[37]  Paul Sajda,et al.  Quality of evidence for perceptual decision making is indexed by trial-to-trial variability of the EEG , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[38]  Norma Wioland,et al.  Cortical Information Processing in Coma , 2009, Cognitive and behavioral neurology : official journal of the Society for Behavioral and Cognitive Neurology.

[39]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  The mismatch negativity: A review of underlying mechanisms , 2009, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[40]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Neural signature of the conscious processing of auditory regularities , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[41]  Steven Laureys,et al.  Mismatch negativity to the patient’s own name in chronic disorders of consciousness , 2008, Neuroscience Letters.

[42]  Dominique Morlet,et al.  Novelty P3 elicited by the subject’s own name in comatose patients , 2008, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[43]  Klaus-Robert Müller,et al.  Machine learning for real-time single-trial EEG-analysis: From brain–computer interfacing to mental state monitoring , 2008, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[44]  L. Naccache,et al.  Auditory mismatch negativity is a good predictor of awakening in comatose patients: a fast and reliable procedure , 2005, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[45]  J. Giacino,et al.  The JFK Coma Recovery Scale-Revised: measurement characteristics and diagnostic utility. , 2004, Archives of physical medicine and rehabilitation.

[46]  D Morlet,et al.  Mismatch negativity and late auditory evoked potentials in comatose patients , 1999, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[47]  S. Wolf,et al.  The quality of evidence. , 1991, Integrative physiological and behavioral science : the official journal of the Pavlovian Society.

[48]  A. Tzovara,et al.  Reply: Neural detection of complex sound sequences or of statistical regularities in the absence of consciousness? , 2015, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[49]  M. D. Lucia,et al.  Prognostic Use of Cognitive Event-Related Potentials in Acute Consciousness Impairment , 2015 .

[50]  Lucas Spierer,et al.  Progression of auditory discrimination based on neural decoding predicts awakening from coma. , 2013, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[51]  C. Fischer,et al.  Sensory and cognitive evoked potentials in the prognosis of coma. , 2004, Supplements to Clinical neurophysiology.