An ERP investigation of the co-development of hemispheric lateralization of face and word recognition

The adult human brain would appear to have specialized and independent neural systems for the visual processing of words and faces. Extensive evidence has demonstrated greater selectivity for written words in the left over right hemisphere, and, conversely, greater selectivity for faces in the right over left hemisphere. This study examines the emergence of these complementary neural profiles, as well as the possible relationship between them. Using behavioral and neurophysiological measures, in adults, we observed the standard finding of greater accuracy and a larger N170 ERP component in the left over right hemisphere for words, and conversely, greater accuracy and a larger N170 in the right over the left hemisphere for faces. We also found that although children aged 7-12 years revealed the adult hemispheric pattern for words, they showed neither a behavioral nor a neural hemispheric superiority for faces. Of particular interest, the magnitude of their N170 for faces in the right hemisphere was related to that of the N170 for words in their left hemisphere. These findings suggest that the hemispheric organization of face recognition and of word recognition does not develop independently, and that word lateralization may precede and drive later face lateralization. A theoretical account for the findings, in which competition for visual representations unfolds over the course of development, is discussed.

[1]  C. Price,et al.  The Interactive Account of ventral occipitotemporal contributions to reading , 2011, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[2]  Daphne Maurer,et al.  Discrimination of Facial Features by Adults, 10-Year-Olds, and Cataract-Reversal Patients , 2010, Perception.

[3]  Talma Hendler,et al.  Eccentricity Bias as an Organizing Principle for Human High-Order Object Areas , 2002, Neuron.

[4]  T. Allison,et al.  Differential Sensitivity of Human Visual Cortex to Faces, Letterstrings, and Textures: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[5]  J Sergent,et al.  Functional and anatomical decomposition of face processing: evidence from prosopagnosia and PET study of normal subjects. , 1992, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[6]  Patricia A. McMullen,et al.  Converging methods for understanding reading and dyslexia , 1999 .

[7]  Ferath Kherif,et al.  Automatic Top-Down Processing Explains Common Left Occipito-Temporal Responses to Visual Words and Objects , 2010, Cerebral cortex.

[8]  M. Seghier,et al.  An anatomical signature for literacy , 2009, Nature.

[9]  S Lehéricy,et al.  The visual word form area: spatial and temporal characterization of an initial stage of reading in normal subjects and posterior split-brain patients. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[10]  J. Cronly-Dillon,et al.  Visual Agnosias and Other Disturbances of Visual Perception and Cognition , 1991 .

[11]  Ying Liu,et al.  Evidence for neural accommodation to a writing system following learning , 2007, Human brain mapping.

[12]  J E Joseph,et al.  Developmental shifts in cortical loci for face and object recognition , 2004, Neuroreport.

[13]  D. Loring,et al.  The occurrence of depressive symptoms in the preclinical phase of AD , 1999, Neurology.

[14]  Bruce D. McCandliss,et al.  Fast, visual specialization for reading in English revealed by the topography of the N170 ERP response , 2005, Behavioral and Brain Functions.

[15]  D. Plaut,et al.  Distributed circuits, not circumscribed centers, mediate visual recognition , 2013, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[16]  Jane E Joseph,et al.  Progressive and regressive developmental changes in neural substrates for face processing: testing specific predictions of the Interactive Specialization account. , 2011, Developmental science.

[17]  D. Plaut,et al.  Complementary neural representations for faces and words: A computational exploration , 2011, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[18]  B. Gelder,et al.  Face processing in adolescents with autistic disorder: The inversion and composite effects , 2003, Brain and Cognition.

[19]  P. Skudlarski,et al.  Disruption of posterior brain systems for reading in children with developmental dyslexia , 2002, Biological Psychiatry.

[20]  Masataka Watanabe,et al.  Human Neuroscience Original Research Article Awareness of Central Luminance Edge Is Crucial for the Craik-o'brien-cornsweet Effect , 2022 .

[21]  Paul M Matthews,et al.  The Role of the Posterior Fusiform Gyrus in Reading , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[22]  K. Grill-Spector,et al.  Differential development of high-level visual cortex correlates with category-specific recognition memory , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[23]  Mark H. Johnson,et al.  The N170 Shows Differential Repetition Effects for Faces, Objects, and Orthographic Stimuli , 2011, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[24]  Cindy M. Bukach,et al.  Holistic Processing of Words Modulated by Reading Experience , 2011, PloS one.

[25]  Daniel Brandeis,et al.  Emerging Neurophysiological Specialization for Letter Strings , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[26]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  Visual word processing and experiential origins of functional selectivity in human extrastriate cortex , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[27]  A. Toga,et al.  Mapping brain asymmetry , 2003, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[28]  Joseph B. Hellige,et al.  Processing Asymmetries in the Visual System , 2010 .

[29]  S. Dehaene,et al.  How Learning to Read Changes the Cortical Networks for Vision and Language , 2010, Science.

[30]  S. Carey,et al.  Developmental changes in the representation of faces. , 1977, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[31]  Kristina M. Visscher,et al.  Functional Neuroanatomical Differences Between Adults and School-Age Children in the Processing of Single Words , 2002, Science.

[32]  Cathy J. Price,et al.  Top-down modulation of ventral occipito-temporal responses during visual word recognition , 2011, NeuroImage.

[33]  Marlene Behrmann,et al.  The joint development of hemispheric lateralization for words and faces. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[34]  N. Kanwisher Domain specificity in face perception , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[35]  J. Sergent,et al.  Functional neuroanatomy of face and object processing. A positron emission tomography study. , 1992, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[36]  Xuchu Weng,et al.  Neural competition as a developmental process: Early hemispheric specialization for word processing delays specialization for face processing , 2013, Neuropsychologia.

[37]  C. Nelson,et al.  Featural and Configural Face Processing in Adults and Infants: A Behavioral and Electrophysiological Investigation , 2006, Perception.

[38]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Specialization within the ventral stream: the case for the visual word form area , 2004, NeuroImage.

[39]  Bruno Rossion,et al.  Cerebral lateralization of face-sensitive areas in left-handers: Only the FFA does not get it right , 2013, Cortex.

[40]  Beatriz Luna,et al.  Visual category-selectivity for faces, places and objects emerges along different developmental trajectories. , 2007, Developmental science.

[41]  Frederic Dick,et al.  Developmental Changes in Effective Connectivity in the Emerging Core Face Network , 2010, Cerebral cortex.

[42]  Bruno Rossion,et al.  Category Specificity in Early Perception: Face and Word N170 Responses Differ in Both Lateralization and Habituation Properties , 2008, Frontiers in human neuroscience.

[43]  Anil Bollimunta,et al.  Local computation of decision-relevant net sensory evidence in parietal cortex. , 2012, Cerebral cortex.

[44]  Andrew N. Meltzoff,et al.  Brain Activation during Face Perception: Evidence of a Developmental Change , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[45]  Kenneth Hugdahl,et al.  The two halves of the brain : information processing in the cerebral hemispheres , 2010 .

[46]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  Location and spatial profile of category‐specific regions in human extrastriate cortex , 2006, Human brain mapping.

[47]  Jyotsna Vaid,et al.  Bilingual language lateralization: A meta-analytic tale of two hemispheres , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[48]  Philippe Pinel,et al.  Cortical representations of symbols, objects, and faces are pruned back during early childhood. , 2011, Cerebral cortex.

[49]  Talma Hendler,et al.  Center–periphery organization of human object areas , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[50]  L. Katz,et al.  Laterality and reading proficiency. , 1974, Neuropsychologia.

[51]  Michael N Jones,et al.  Case-sensitive letter and bigram frequency counts from large-scale English corpora , 2004, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.

[52]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  The Neural Basis of the Behavioral Face-Inversion Effect , 2005, Current Biology.

[53]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  The Fusiform Face Area: A Module in Human Extrastriate Cortex Specialized for Face Perception , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[54]  I. Gauthier,et al.  A defense of the subordinate-level expertise account for the N170 component , 2002, Cognition.

[55]  HighWire Press Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London , 1781, The London Medical Journal.