Age-related decrements in Stroop Color Test performance.

Administered a modified Stroop Color-Word Test to 80 healthy males aged 21-90 years as part of a study of neuropsychological functioning. No age differences were demonstrated on simple reading tasks; however, significant age effects were observed for the color naming and interference tasks (ps less than .001). Ss in the oldest age groups (61-70 and 71-90 years) performed more slowly than younger individuals on both of these. Of a variety of tests administered, performance on the Stroop Test was most affected by age. Because impairment on Stroop color naming and interference tests appears to be a concomitant of normal aging, caution should be exercised when one is interpreting Stroop test results from older patients with suspected cerebral dysfunction.

[1]  C. Shagass Evoked Brain Potentials in Psychiatry , 1972, Springer US.

[2]  C. Dodrill A Neuropsychological Battery for Epilepsy , 1978, Epilepsia.

[3]  M. Bando,et al.  Changes in Blue Fluorescence Intensity and Coloration of Human Lens Protein with Normal Lens Aging and Nuclear Cataract , 1976 .

[4]  R. E. Dustman,et al.  Memory-For-Designs Test: comparison of performance of young and old adults. , 1980, Journal of clinical psychology.

[5]  H. Werner,et al.  Interference effects of Stroop color-word test in childhood, adulthood, and aging. , 1962, The Journal of genetic psychology.

[6]  John M. Davis,et al.  Changes in Monoamine Oxidase with Aging , 1973 .

[7]  R. Nebes,et al.  Vocal versus manual response as a determinant of age difference in simple reaction time. , 1978, Journal of gerontology.

[8]  L. Jarvik,et al.  Stroop color-word test, non-psychotic organic brain syndrome, and chromosome loss in aged twins. , 1971, Journal of gerontology.

[9]  C. Golden,et al.  Identification of brain disorders by the Stroop Color and Word Test. , 1976, Journal of clinical psychology.

[10]  Arnold B. Scheibel,et al.  Structural Changes in the Aging Brain , 1975 .

[11]  J. Botwinick,et al.  Sensory-set factors in age differences in reaction time. , 1971, The Journal of genetic psychology.

[12]  T. Schenkenberg,et al.  NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL CORRELATES OF CHRONIC ALCOHOLISM AND AGING , 1977, The Journal of nervous and mental disease.

[13]  E. W. Snyder,et al.  Life-span changes in visually evoked potentials at central scalp , 1981, Neurobiology of Aging.

[14]  Charles H. M. Beck Functional Implications of Changes in the Senescent Brain: A Review , 1978, Canadian Journal of Neurological Sciences / Journal Canadien des Sciences Neurologiques.

[15]  P. E. Comalli COGNITIVE FUNCTIONING IN A GROUP OF 80-90-YEAR-OLD MEN. , 1965, Journal of gerontology.

[16]  E. W. Snyder,et al.  Life-span alterations in visually evoked potentials and inhibitory function , 1981, Neurobiology of Aging.

[17]  S. Wapner,et al.  COGNITIVE FUNCTIONING IN TWO GROUPS OF AGED: ONE INSTITUTIONALIZED, THE OTHER LIVING IN THE COMMUNITY. , 1965, Journal of gerontology.

[18]  Jack Botwinick,et al.  Aging and behavior;: A comprehensive integration of research findings , 1973 .

[19]  Graham Fk,et al.  Performance of brain-damaged cases on a memory-for-designs test. , 1946 .

[20]  P. Mcgeer,et al.  Chemistry of mood and emotion. , 1980, Annual review of psychology.

[21]  E. Ajax,et al.  Comparison of different scoring and administration procedures for the memory‐for‐designs test , 1970 .

[22]  S. Kent Neurotransmitters may be weak link in the aging brain's communication network. , 1976, Geriatrics.