Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin Freitas Et Al. / Regulatory Focus and Action Initiation When to Begin? Regulatory Focus and Initiating Goal Pursuit

The authors propose that a prevention focus fosters preferences to initiate action earlier than does a promotion focus. Data from four studies either measuring or manipulating regulatory focus support this proposal. Participants in a prevention focus preferred initiating academic (Studies 1 and 2) and nonacademic (Study 3) actions sooner than did participants in a promotion focus. Participants working through a set of anagrams solved those that were prevention framed before those that were promotion framed (Study 4). Moreover, regulatory focus and perceived task valence each accounted for unique variance in participants’ task-initiation preferences (Study 3). The findings’ implications are discussed for task choice, susceptibility to distraction, and other aspects of self-regulation.

[1]  E. Higgins Promotion and Prevention: Regulatory Focus as A Motivational Principle , 1998 .

[2]  J. N. Bassili Meta-judgmental versus operative indexes of psychological attributes: The case of measures of attitude strength. , 1996 .

[3]  P. Gollwitzer The Volitional Benefits of Planning , 1996 .

[4]  N. Lewis A dynamic theory of personality , 1935 .

[5]  R. Gould An experimental analysis of "level of aspiration" , 1939 .

[6]  Peter Salovey,et al.  The Quasi-Acceleration Relation: Satisfaction as a Function of the Change of Velocity of Outcome Over Time , 2008 .

[7]  G. Loewenstein,et al.  Do Workers Prefer Increasing Wage Profiles? , 1991, Journal of Labor Economics.

[8]  A. Tversky,et al.  Prospect theory: analysis of decision under risk , 1979 .

[9]  E. Higgins,et al.  Principles of Judging Valence: What Makes Events Positive or Negative? , 1996 .

[10]  C. Carver,et al.  A control-process perspective on the origins of affect. , 1996 .

[11]  Itamar Simonson,et al.  Evaluations of pairs of experiences: A preference for happy endings , 1991 .

[12]  Y. I. Kim [General systems theory]. , 1989, Taehan kanho. The Korean nurse.

[13]  Russell H. Fazio,et al.  A practical guide to the use of response latency in social psychological research. , 1990 .

[14]  E. Higgins,et al.  Beyond pleasure and pain. , 1997, The American psychologist.

[15]  John B. Carroll,et al.  The American Heritage Word Frequency Book , 1971 .

[16]  Ira J. Roseman Appraisal determinants of discrete emotions. , 1991 .

[17]  E. Higgins,et al.  Expectancy x value effects: regulatory focus as determinant of magnitude and direction. , 1997, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[18]  R. C. Silver,et al.  The myths of coping with loss. , 1989, Journal of consulting and clinical psychology.

[19]  The effect of normative beliefs on anticipated emotions. , 1992, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[20]  J. A. Wiehe,et al.  From wishing to intending: differences in salience of positive versus negative consequences. , 1994, Journal of personality.

[21]  R. Herrnstein Rational Choice Theory Necessary but Not Sufficient , 1990 .

[22]  C. Carver,et al.  Attention and Self-Regulation: A Control-Theory Approach to Human Behavior , 1981 .

[23]  G. Loewenstein Anticipation and the Valuation of Delayed Consumption , 1987 .

[24]  E. Higgins,et al.  Handbook of motivation and cognition : foundations of social behavior , 1991 .

[25]  Robert P. Abelson,et al.  Velocity relation : satisfaction as a function of the first derivative of outcome over time , 1991 .

[26]  N. Liberman,et al.  Distinguishing Gains from Nonlosses and Losses from Nongains: A Regulatory Focus Perspective on Hedonic Intensity , 2000 .

[27]  C. Carver,et al.  Origins and Functions of Positive and Negative Affect: A Control-Process View. , 1990 .

[28]  E. Higgins,et al.  Emotional responses to goal attainment: strength of regulatory focus as moderator. , 1997, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[29]  R. Thaler,et al.  Gambling with the house money and trying to break even: the effects of prior outcomes on risky choice , 1990 .

[30]  E. Higgins,et al.  Ideal versus ought predilections for approach and avoidance: distinct self-regulatory systems. , 1994, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[31]  G. Loewenstein,et al.  Preferences for sequences of outcomes. , 1993 .

[32]  G. W. Fischer,et al.  Preferences for separating or combining events. , 1991, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[33]  J. Bargh,et al.  The psychology of action : linking cognition and motivation to behavior , 1999 .

[34]  J. N. Bassili,et al.  Response Latency and the Accessibility of Voting Intentions: What Contributes to Accessibility and How it Affects Vote Choice , 1995 .

[35]  M. Banaji,et al.  Implicit social cognition: attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. , 1995, Psychological review.

[36]  Alexander J. Rothman,et al.  Shaping perceptions to motivate healthy behavior: the role of message framing. , 1997, Psychological bulletin.

[37]  Lawrence A. Pervin,et al.  Goal concepts in personality and social psychology. , 1989 .

[38]  R. Fazio On the automatic activation of associated evaluations: An overview , 2001 .

[39]  Daniel Kahneman,et al.  Experiences extended across time: Evaluation of moments and episodes , 1992 .

[40]  Michael A. Becker Social Psychology: Handbook of Basic Principles , 1998 .

[41]  J. Forgas Feeling and thinking : the role of affect in social cognition , 2000 .

[42]  N Liberman,et al.  Promotion and prevention choices between stability and change. , 1999, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[43]  Samuel Kotz,et al.  Discrete Distributions: Distributions in Statistics , 1971 .

[44]  G. Miller,et al.  Plans and the structure of behavior , 1960 .

[45]  P. Brickman,et al.  Lottery winners and accident victims: is happiness relative? , 1978, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[46]  W. Mischel,et al.  Effects of temptation-inhibiting and task-facilitating plans on self-control. , 1976 .

[47]  E. Higgins,et al.  Performance incentives and means: how regulatory focus influences goal attainment. , 1998, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[48]  M. G. Preston,et al.  Differential effect of a social variable upon three levels of aspiration , 1941 .

[49]  Erika D Peterson,et al.  Temporal Adjustments in the Evaluation of Events: The "Rosy View" , 1997, Journal of experimental social psychology.

[50]  A. Battersby Plans and the Structure of Behavior , 1968 .

[51]  G. Vining,et al.  Data Analysis: A Model-Comparison Approach , 1989 .

[52]  E. Higgins,et al.  Self-Concept Discrepancy Theory: A Psychological Model for Distinguishing among Different Aspects of Depression and Anxiety , 1985 .

[53]  The Negative Hypergeometric Probability Distribution: Sampling without Replacement from a Finite Population , 1998 .

[54]  N. Liberman,et al.  Approach and avoidance strength during goal attainment: regulatory focus and the "goal looms larger" effect. , 1998, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[55]  Mancur Olson,et al.  Positive Time Preference , 1981, Journal of Political Economy.

[56]  Alexander J. Rothman,et al.  Message framing and sunscreen use: gain-framed messages motivate beach-goers. , 1999, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[57]  R. Fazio How do attitudes guide behavior , 1986 .

[58]  Thomas D. Cook,et al.  Subject effects in laboratory research: An examination of subject roles, demand characteristics, and valid inference. , 1972 .

[59]  B. Wampold,et al.  Nonparametric strategies to test the independence of behavioral states in sequential data. , 1982 .