The influence of training views on infants' long-term memory for simple 3D shapes.

This investigation explored infants' ability to retrieve a memory for a simple 3D shape from a novel view following a 24-hr delay. Tests of memory for shape in infancy have typically used extremely short delay intervals between familiarization and test in examining the ability to equate between substantially different views of a 3D object. The current study used longer delays to assess the content of a long-term memory representation. Infants 3-4 months of age learned to kick to move a mobile displaying a simple 3D shape (brick or cylinder). Results of three experiments show that infants can recognize 3D shapes in a novel viewpoint across a 24-hr delay, provided that experience with a sufficiently wide range of views is available during training. The results suggest a capacity for the perception of 3D shape that enables access, across long delays, to a memory representation of sufficient detail that discrimination between two simple shapes (i.e., a cylinder and a brick) is possible. The results suggest that this representation is of a sufficiently abstract nature that perception of the 3D form of the object, independent of the changes in specific features accompanying changes in viewpoint, is also possible. This finding suggests that infants, like adults, possess a functional memory system for the distal shape of simple 3D objects, and can transfer training to a novel view using long-term memory, but that this ability is not as strong as in the mature system. These results have implications for the development of shape perception and for theories of object recognition in general.

[1]  I. Gauthier,et al.  Visual object understanding , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[2]  P. Gerhardstein,et al.  New methodology in infant operant kicking procedures: computerized stimulus control and computerized measurement of kicking , 2004 .

[3]  D. Foster,et al.  Recognizing novel three–dimensional objects by summing signals from parts and views , 2002, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[4]  Brian J Stankiewicz,et al.  Empirical evidence for independent dimensions in the visual representation of three-dimensional shape. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[5]  Scott P. Johnson,et al.  Young infants' perception of unity and form in occlusion displays. , 2002, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[6]  D. Mareschal Object knowledge in infancy: current controversies and approaches , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[7]  A. Yonas,et al.  Perception of three-dimensional shape specified by optic flow by 8-week-old infants , 2000, Perception & psychophysics.

[8]  C. Rovee-Collier,et al.  A dissociation in infants' memory for stimulus size: evidence for the early development of multiple memory systems. , 2000, Developmental psychobiology.

[9]  Carolyn Rovee-Collier,et al.  The Development of Implicit and Explicit Memory , 2000 .

[10]  M J Tarr,et al.  Recognizing Silhouettes and Shaded Images across Depth Rotation , 1999, Perception.

[11]  C. J. Marsolek Dissociable Neural Subsystems Underlie Abstract and Specific Object Recognition , 1999 .

[12]  S. Ullman Three-dimensional object recognition based on the combination of views , 1998, Cognition.

[13]  Heinrich H Bülthoff,et al.  Image-based object recognition in man, monkey and machine , 1998, Cognition.

[14]  M. Haith Who put the cog in infant cognition ? Is rich interpretation too costly ? , 1998 .

[15]  E. Spelke Nativism, empiricism, and the origins of knowledge , 1998 .

[16]  Jane L. Abraham,et al.  The development of infants' preference for motherese , 1997 .

[17]  C. Rovee-Collier,et al.  Dissociation between features and feature relations in infant memory: effects of memory load. , 1997, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[18]  D. Lewkowicz Infants' Response to the Audible and Visible Properties of the Human Face: 1. Role of Lexical-Syntactic Content, Temporal Synchrony, Gender, and Manner of Speech , 1996 .

[19]  C. Rovee-Collier,et al.  Infants' forgetting of correlated attributes and object recognition. , 1996, Child development.

[20]  J. Hummel,et al.  An architecture for rapid, hierarchical structural description , 1996 .

[21]  David L. Sheinberg,et al.  Visual object recognition. , 1996, Annual review of neuroscience.

[22]  I. Biederman,et al.  Viewpoint-dependent mechanisms in visual object recognition: Reply to Tarr and Bülthoff (1995). , 1995 .

[23]  N. Logothetis,et al.  Psychophysical and physiological evidence for viewer-centered object representations in the primate. , 1995, Cerebral cortex.

[24]  C. Rovee-Collier,et al.  Substituting new details for old? Effects of delaying postevent information on infant memory , 1994, Memory & cognition.

[25]  C. Rovee-Collier,et al.  Developmental Changes in the Interface between Perception and Memory Retrieval. , 1994 .

[26]  T. Poggio,et al.  The importance of symmetry and virtual views in three-dimensional object recognition , 1994, Current Biology.

[27]  C. Rovee-Collier,et al.  Perception and 24-Hour Retention of Feature Relations in Infancy. , 1994 .

[28]  I. Biederman,et al.  Recognizing depth-rotated objects: evidence and conditions for three-dimensional viewpoint invariance. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[29]  I. Biederman,et al.  Size invariance in visual object priming , 1992 .

[30]  D L Schacter,et al.  Priming and recognition of transformed three-dimensional objects: effects of size and reflection. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[31]  T. Poggio,et al.  A network that learns to recognize three-dimensional objects , 1990, Nature.

[32]  I. Biederman Recognition-by-components: a theory of human image understanding. , 1987, Psychological review.

[33]  Shinsuke Shimojo,et al.  Pre-stereoptic binocular vision in infants , 1986, Vision Research.

[34]  H. Hayne,et al.  Ontogeny of early event memory: I. Forgetting and retrieval by 2- and 3-month-olds , 1986 .

[35]  M. Bornstein,et al.  Fine orientation discrimination and shape constancy in young infants. , 1986, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[36]  A. Slater,et al.  Shape Constancy and Slant Perception at Birth , 1985, Perception.

[37]  A. Fernald Four-Month-Old Infants Prefer to Listen to Motherese" , 1985 .

[38]  Infant perception of the shapes of tilted plane forms , 1984 .

[39]  P. Kellman Perception of three-dimensional form by human infants , 1984, Perception & psychophysics.

[40]  S. Dumais,et al.  Stereopsis in human infants. , 1980, Science.

[41]  V. R. Carlson,et al.  Infant perception of the invariant shape of objects varying in slant. , 1979, Child development.

[42]  C. Rovee-Collier,et al.  The economics of infancy: a review of conjugate reinforcement. , 1979, Advances in child development and behavior.

[43]  M. Cook,et al.  The perception of solid form in early infancy. , 1978, Child development.

[44]  H. Ruff Infant recognition of the invariant form of objects. , 1978, Child development.

[45]  T. Allen,et al.  Role of object constancy in the perception of object orientation. Some methodological considerations in studies of human infants. , 1974, Human development.

[46]  C. K. Rovee,et al.  Conjugate reinforcement of infant exploratory behavior. , 1969, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[47]  T G Bower,et al.  The visual world of infants. , 1966, Scientific American.