Emergent Synergistic Grasp-Like Behavior in a Visuomotor Joint Action Task: Evidence for Internal Forward Models as Building Blocks of Human Interactions

Central to the mechanistic understanding of the human mind is to clarify how cognitive functions arise from simpler sensory and motor functions. A longstanding assumption is that forward models used by sensorimotor control to anticipate actions also serve to incorporate other people’s actions and intentions, and give rise to sensorimotor interactions between people, and even abstract forms of interactions. That is, forward models could aid core aspects of human social cognition. To test whether forward models can be used to coordinate interactions, here we measured the movements of pairs of participants in a novel joint action task. For the task they collaborated to lift an object, each of them using fingers of one hand to push against the object from opposite sides, just like a single person would use two hands to grasp the object bimanually. Perturbations of the object were applied randomly as they are known to impact grasp-specific movement components in common grasping tasks. We found that co-actors quickly learned to make grasp-like movements with grasp components that showed coordination on average based on action observation of peak deviation and velocity of their partner’s trajectories. Our data suggest that co-actors adopted pre-existing bimanual grasp programs for their own body to use forward models of their partner’s effectors. This is consistent with the long-held assumption that human higher-order cognitive functions may take advantage of sensorimotor forward models to plan social behavior. New and Noteworthy: Taking an approach of sensorimotor neuroscience, our work provides evidence for a long-held belief that the coordination of physical as well as abstract interactions between people originates from certain sensorimotor control processes that form mental representations of people’s bodies and actions, called forward models. With a new joint action paradigm and several new analysis approaches we show that, indeed, people coordinate each other’s interactions based on forward models and mutual action observation.

[1]  B. Bertenthal,et al.  Does Perception of Biological Motion Rely on Specific Brain Regions? , 2001, NeuroImage.

[2]  Andrew W. Fitzgibbon,et al.  Direct Least Square Fitting of Ellipses , 1999, IEEE Trans. Pattern Anal. Mach. Intell..

[3]  Matteo Candidi,et al.  Causative role of left aIPS in coding shared goals during human–avatar complementary joint actions , 2015, Nature Communications.

[4]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Brain circuits for the internal monitoring of movements. , 2008, Annual review of neuroscience.

[5]  Peter M. Vishton,et al.  Haptically Linked Dyads , 2006, Psychological science.

[6]  David W. Franklin,et al.  Computational Mechanisms of Sensorimotor Control , 2011, Neuron.

[7]  Scott T. Grafton The cognitive neuroscience of prehension: recent developments , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[8]  Luciano Fadiga,et al.  The motor cortex is causally related to predictive eye movements during action observation , 2013, Neuropsychologia.

[9]  Yves Rossetti,et al.  Implicit Short-Lived Motor Representations of Space in Brain Damaged and Healthy Subjects , 1998, Consciousness and Cognition.

[10]  Robrecht P R D van der Wel,et al.  Let the force be with us: dyads exploit haptic coupling for coordination. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[11]  C. Hofsten,et al.  Infants predict other people's action goals , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[12]  M. Davare,et al.  Behavioral / Systems / Cognitive Dissociating the Role of Ventral and Dorsal Premotor Cortex in Precision Grasping , 2018 .

[13]  S. Scott Optimal feedback control and the neural basis of volitional motor control , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[14]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Motor facilitation during action observation: a magnetic stimulation study. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[15]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Formation of a Motor Memory by Action Observation , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[16]  U. Castiello,et al.  Different action patterns for cooperative and competitive behaviour , 2007, Cognition.

[17]  C. Frith,et al.  Interacting minds--a biological basis. , 1999, Science.

[18]  Cristina Becchio,et al.  Grasping others' movements: Rapid discrimination of object size from observed hand movements. , 2016, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[19]  K. Doya,et al.  A unifying computational framework for motor control and social interaction. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[20]  D M Wolpert,et al.  Multiple paired forward and inverse models for motor control , 1998, Neural Networks.

[21]  E. Brenner,et al.  A new view on grasping. , 1999, Motor control.

[22]  Andrew Blake,et al.  Computational modelling of hand-eye coordination , 1992 .

[23]  Eli Brenner,et al.  Size illusion influences how we lift but not how we grasp an object , 1996, Experimental Brain Research.

[24]  Cordula Vesper,et al.  Are you ready to jump? Predictive mechanisms in interpersonal coordination. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[25]  Scott T Grafton,et al.  The Anterior Intraparietal Sulcus Mediates Grasp Execution, Independent of Requirement to Update: New Insights from Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[26]  U. Castiello,et al.  The case of Dr. Jekyll and Mr. Hyde: A kinematic study on social intention , 2008, Consciousness and Cognition.

[27]  P. van Donkelaar,et al.  Predicting Future Sensorimotor States Influences Current Temporal Decision Making , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[28]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  Grasping the Intentions of Others with One's Own Mirror Neuron System , 2005, PLoS biology.

[29]  Matthias Niemeier,et al.  Left visual field preference for a bimanual grasping task with ecologically valid object sizes , 2013, Experimental Brain Research.

[30]  Eric L. Amazeen,et al.  Interpersonal Fitts’ law: when two perform as one , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.

[31]  T. Paus,et al.  Seeing and hearing speech excites the motor system involved in speech production , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[32]  K. Shockley,et al.  Mutual interpersonal postural constraints are involved in cooperative conversation. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[33]  Raoul M. Bongers,et al.  Movements of Individual Digits in Bimanual Prehension Are Coupled into a Grasping Component , 2014, PloS one.

[34]  Natalie Sebanz,et al.  Prediction in Joint Action: What, When, and Where , 2009, Top. Cogn. Sci..

[35]  S. Jackson,et al.  Monocular Vision Leads to a Dissociation between Grip Force and Grip Aperture Scaling during Reach-to-Grasp Movements , 2002, Current Biology.

[36]  G. Knoblich,et al.  Action coordination in groups and individuals: learning anticipatory control. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[37]  W. Medendorp,et al.  Functional magnetic resonance imaging adaptation reveals the cortical networks for processing grasp-relevant object properties. , 2014, Cerebral cortex.

[38]  U. Castiello,et al.  Both your intention and mine are reflected in the kinematics of my reach-to-grasp movement , 2008, Cognition.

[39]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  Optimal feedback control as a theory of motor coordination , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[40]  Matthew Ray,et al.  Response Selection During a Joint Action Task , 2011, Journal of motor behavior.

[41]  Christian Keysers,et al.  Mirror neurons , 2009, Current Biology.

[42]  Ken Nakayama,et al.  Predicting actions from subtle preparatory movements , 2017, Cognition.

[43]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Speech listening specifically modulates the excitability of tongue muscles: a TMS study , 2002, The European journal of neuroscience.

[44]  Matthew F. S. Rushworth,et al.  Parietal rTMS Disrupts the Initiation but not the Execution of On-line Adjustments to a Perturbation of Object Size , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[45]  Matthias Niemeier,et al.  Visual field preferences of object analysis for grasping with one hand , 2014, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[46]  Edgar Erdfelder,et al.  G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences , 2007, Behavior research methods.

[47]  J Randall Flanagan,et al.  Separate Contributions of Kinematic and Kinetic Errors to Trajectory and Grip Force Adaptation When Transporting Novel Hand-Held Loads , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[48]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  Selective perturbation of visual input during prehension movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[49]  R. Miall,et al.  Connecting mirror neurons and forward models. , 2003, Neuroreport.

[50]  K. Lynch,et al.  The Separate Neural Control of Hand Movements and Contact Forces , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[51]  Roberto Danovaro,et al.  Exponential Decline of Deep-Sea Ecosystem Functioning Linked to Benthic Biodiversity Loss , 2008, Current Biology.

[52]  C. Frith,et al.  The Neural Basis of Mentalizing , 2006, Neuron.

[53]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  Cortical mechanisms of human imitation. , 1999, Science.

[54]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Influence of different types of grasping on the transport component of prehension movements , 1991, Neuropsychologia.

[55]  Matthias Niemeier,et al.  Suppression and reversal of motion perception around the time of the saccade , 2015, Front. Syst. Neurosci..

[56]  W. Prinz,et al.  How two share a task: corepresenting stimulus-response mappings. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[57]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Goal Representation in Human Anterior Intraparietal Sulcus , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[58]  J. Kelso,et al.  Social coordination dynamics: Measuring human bonding , 2008, Social neuroscience.

[59]  Matthias Niemeier,et al.  The right anterior intraparietal sulcus is critical for bimanual grasping: a TMS study. , 2014, Cerebral cortex.

[60]  Michael J Richardson,et al.  Horsing Around: Spontaneous Four-Legged Coordination , 2009, Journal of motor behavior.

[61]  Matthias Niemeier,et al.  Parietal area BA7 integrates motor programs for reaching, grasping, and bimanual coordination. , 2017, Journal of neurophysiology.

[62]  Mark L. Latash,et al.  Mirror Writing: Learning, Transfer, and Implications for Internal Inverse Models. , 1999, Journal of motor behavior.

[63]  Yasuhiro Kanakogi,et al.  Developmental correspondence between action prediction and motor ability in early infancy. , 2011, Nature communications.

[64]  Y. Paulignan,et al.  An Interference Effect of Observed Biological Movement on Action , 2003, Current Biology.

[65]  R. Johansson,et al.  Action plans used in action observation , 2003, Nature.

[66]  Vladimir M. Zatsiorsky,et al.  Interpersonal synergies: static prehension tasks performed by two actors , 2016, Experimental Brain Research.

[67]  M. Turvey,et al.  Phase transitions and critical fluctuations in the visual coordination of rhythmic movements between people. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[68]  W. Prinz,et al.  Representing others' actions: just like one's own? , 2003, Cognition.

[69]  Timothy D. Lee,et al.  Schema Theory: Critical Review and Implications for the Role of Cognition in a New Theory of Motor Learning , 2003, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.

[70]  Andrew W. Fitzgibbon,et al.  Direct least squares fitting of ellipses , 1996, Proceedings of 13th International Conference on Pattern Recognition.

[71]  G. Pezzulo,et al.  Interactional leader–follower sensorimotor communication strategies during repetitive joint actions , 2015, Journal of The Royal Society Interface.

[72]  Giulio Sandini,et al.  Understanding Object Weight from Human and Humanoid Lifting Actions , 2014, IEEE Transactions on Autonomous Mental Development.

[73]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[74]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Virtual lesions of the anterior intraparietal area disrupt goal-dependent on-line adjustments of grasp , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[75]  Daniel Jokisch,et al.  Self recognition versus recognition of others by biological motion: viewpoint-dependent effects. , 2004, Perception.

[76]  M. Arbib,et al.  Language within our grasp , 1998, Trends in Neurosciences.

[77]  M. Candidi,et al.  Kinematics fingerprints of leader and follower role-taking during cooperative joint actions , 2013, Experimental Brain Research.

[78]  Denise Y. P. Henriques,et al.  Visuomotor adaptation and generalization with repeated and varied training , 2013, Experimental Brain Research.

[79]  G. Johansson Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis , 1973 .

[80]  付伶俐 打磨Using Language,倡导新理念 , 2014 .

[81]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Action recognition in the premotor cortex. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[82]  Derek Hoiem,et al.  Action Recognition , 2014, Computer Vision, A Reference Guide.

[83]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Activation of human primary motor cortex during action observation: a neuromagnetic study. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[84]  L. Fadiga,et al.  Sensorimotor communication in professional quartets , 2014, Neuropsychologia.

[85]  M. A. Goodale,et al.  Factors affecting higher-order movement planning: a kinematic analysis of human prehension , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[86]  Janeen D. Loehr,et al.  Monitoring Individual and Joint Action Outcomes in Duet Music Performance , 2013, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[87]  Matthias Niemeier,et al.  A right hemisphere dominance for bimanual grasps , 2012, Experimental Brain Research.

[88]  Luca Turella,et al.  Mirror neurons in humans: Consisting or confounding evidence? , 2009, Brain and Language.

[89]  V. Gallese The Roots of Empathy: The Shared Manifold Hypothesis and the Neural Basis of Intersubjectivity , 2003, Psychopathology.

[90]  M. Jeannerod The timing of natural prehension movements. , 1984, Journal of motor behavior.

[91]  Ravi S. Menon,et al.  Visually guided grasping produces fMRI activation in dorsal but not ventral stream brain areas , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.