Emergent Synergistic Grasp-Like Behavior in a Visuomotor Joint Action Task: Evidence for Internal Forward Models as Building Blocks of Human Interactions
暂无分享,去创建一个
Matthias Niemeier | Lin Lawrence Guo | Namita Patel | M. Niemeier | L. Guo | Namita Patel | Matthias Niemeier
[1] B. Bertenthal,et al. Does Perception of Biological Motion Rely on Specific Brain Regions? , 2001, NeuroImage.
[2] Andrew W. Fitzgibbon,et al. Direct Least Square Fitting of Ellipses , 1999, IEEE Trans. Pattern Anal. Mach. Intell..
[3] Matteo Candidi,et al. Causative role of left aIPS in coding shared goals during human–avatar complementary joint actions , 2015, Nature Communications.
[4] R. Wurtz,et al. Brain circuits for the internal monitoring of movements. , 2008, Annual review of neuroscience.
[5] Peter M. Vishton,et al. Haptically Linked Dyads , 2006, Psychological science.
[6] David W. Franklin,et al. Computational Mechanisms of Sensorimotor Control , 2011, Neuron.
[7] Scott T. Grafton. The cognitive neuroscience of prehension: recent developments , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.
[8] Luciano Fadiga,et al. The motor cortex is causally related to predictive eye movements during action observation , 2013, Neuropsychologia.
[9] Yves Rossetti,et al. Implicit Short-Lived Motor Representations of Space in Brain Damaged and Healthy Subjects , 1998, Consciousness and Cognition.
[10] Robrecht P R D van der Wel,et al. Let the force be with us: dyads exploit haptic coupling for coordination. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[11] C. Hofsten,et al. Infants predict other people's action goals , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.
[12] M. Davare,et al. Behavioral / Systems / Cognitive Dissociating the Role of Ventral and Dorsal Premotor Cortex in Precision Grasping , 2018 .
[13] S. Scott. Optimal feedback control and the neural basis of volitional motor control , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[14] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Motor facilitation during action observation: a magnetic stimulation study. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.
[15] Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al. Formation of a Motor Memory by Action Observation , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[16] U. Castiello,et al. Different action patterns for cooperative and competitive behaviour , 2007, Cognition.
[17] C. Frith,et al. Interacting minds--a biological basis. , 1999, Science.
[18] Cristina Becchio,et al. Grasping others' movements: Rapid discrimination of object size from observed hand movements. , 2016, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[19] K. Doya,et al. A unifying computational framework for motor control and social interaction. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.
[20] D M Wolpert,et al. Multiple paired forward and inverse models for motor control , 1998, Neural Networks.
[21] E. Brenner,et al. A new view on grasping. , 1999, Motor control.
[22] Andrew Blake,et al. Computational modelling of hand-eye coordination , 1992 .
[23] Eli Brenner,et al. Size illusion influences how we lift but not how we grasp an object , 1996, Experimental Brain Research.
[24] Cordula Vesper,et al. Are you ready to jump? Predictive mechanisms in interpersonal coordination. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[25] Scott T Grafton,et al. The Anterior Intraparietal Sulcus Mediates Grasp Execution, Independent of Requirement to Update: New Insights from Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[26] U. Castiello,et al. The case of Dr. Jekyll and Mr. Hyde: A kinematic study on social intention , 2008, Consciousness and Cognition.
[27] P. van Donkelaar,et al. Predicting Future Sensorimotor States Influences Current Temporal Decision Making , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[28] J. Mazziotta,et al. Grasping the Intentions of Others with One's Own Mirror Neuron System , 2005, PLoS biology.
[29] Matthias Niemeier,et al. Left visual field preference for a bimanual grasping task with ecologically valid object sizes , 2013, Experimental Brain Research.
[30] Eric L. Amazeen,et al. Interpersonal Fitts’ law: when two perform as one , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.
[31] T. Paus,et al. Seeing and hearing speech excites the motor system involved in speech production , 2003, Neuropsychologia.
[32] K. Shockley,et al. Mutual interpersonal postural constraints are involved in cooperative conversation. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[33] Raoul M. Bongers,et al. Movements of Individual Digits in Bimanual Prehension Are Coupled into a Grasping Component , 2014, PloS one.
[34] Natalie Sebanz,et al. Prediction in Joint Action: What, When, and Where , 2009, Top. Cogn. Sci..
[35] S. Jackson,et al. Monocular Vision Leads to a Dissociation between Grip Force and Grip Aperture Scaling during Reach-to-Grasp Movements , 2002, Current Biology.
[36] G. Knoblich,et al. Action coordination in groups and individuals: learning anticipatory control. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[37] W. Medendorp,et al. Functional magnetic resonance imaging adaptation reveals the cortical networks for processing grasp-relevant object properties. , 2014, Cerebral cortex.
[38] U. Castiello,et al. Both your intention and mine are reflected in the kinematics of my reach-to-grasp movement , 2008, Cognition.
[39] Michael I. Jordan,et al. Optimal feedback control as a theory of motor coordination , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.
[40] Matthew Ray,et al. Response Selection During a Joint Action Task , 2011, Journal of motor behavior.
[41] Christian Keysers,et al. Mirror neurons , 2009, Current Biology.
[42] Ken Nakayama,et al. Predicting actions from subtle preparatory movements , 2017, Cognition.
[43] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Speech listening specifically modulates the excitability of tongue muscles: a TMS study , 2002, The European journal of neuroscience.
[44] Matthew F. S. Rushworth,et al. Parietal rTMS Disrupts the Initiation but not the Execution of On-line Adjustments to a Perturbation of Object Size , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[45] Matthias Niemeier,et al. Visual field preferences of object analysis for grasping with one hand , 2014, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[46] Edgar Erdfelder,et al. G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences , 2007, Behavior research methods.
[47] J Randall Flanagan,et al. Separate Contributions of Kinematic and Kinetic Errors to Trajectory and Grip Force Adaptation When Transporting Novel Hand-Held Loads , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[48] M. Jeannerod,et al. Selective perturbation of visual input during prehension movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[49] R. Miall,et al. Connecting mirror neurons and forward models. , 2003, Neuroreport.
[50] K. Lynch,et al. The Separate Neural Control of Hand Movements and Contact Forces , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[51] Roberto Danovaro,et al. Exponential Decline of Deep-Sea Ecosystem Functioning Linked to Benthic Biodiversity Loss , 2008, Current Biology.
[52] C. Frith,et al. The Neural Basis of Mentalizing , 2006, Neuron.
[53] J. Mazziotta,et al. Cortical mechanisms of human imitation. , 1999, Science.
[54] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Influence of different types of grasping on the transport component of prehension movements , 1991, Neuropsychologia.
[55] Matthias Niemeier,et al. Suppression and reversal of motion perception around the time of the saccade , 2015, Front. Syst. Neurosci..
[56] W. Prinz,et al. How two share a task: corepresenting stimulus-response mappings. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[57] Scott T. Grafton,et al. Goal Representation in Human Anterior Intraparietal Sulcus , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[58] J. Kelso,et al. Social coordination dynamics: Measuring human bonding , 2008, Social neuroscience.
[59] Matthias Niemeier,et al. The right anterior intraparietal sulcus is critical for bimanual grasping: a TMS study. , 2014, Cerebral cortex.
[60] Michael J Richardson,et al. Horsing Around: Spontaneous Four-Legged Coordination , 2009, Journal of motor behavior.
[61] Matthias Niemeier,et al. Parietal area BA7 integrates motor programs for reaching, grasping, and bimanual coordination. , 2017, Journal of neurophysiology.
[62] Mark L. Latash,et al. Mirror Writing: Learning, Transfer, and Implications for Internal Inverse Models. , 1999, Journal of motor behavior.
[63] Yasuhiro Kanakogi,et al. Developmental correspondence between action prediction and motor ability in early infancy. , 2011, Nature communications.
[64] Y. Paulignan,et al. An Interference Effect of Observed Biological Movement on Action , 2003, Current Biology.
[65] R. Johansson,et al. Action plans used in action observation , 2003, Nature.
[66] Vladimir M. Zatsiorsky,et al. Interpersonal synergies: static prehension tasks performed by two actors , 2016, Experimental Brain Research.
[67] M. Turvey,et al. Phase transitions and critical fluctuations in the visual coordination of rhythmic movements between people. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[68] W. Prinz,et al. Representing others' actions: just like one's own? , 2003, Cognition.
[69] Timothy D. Lee,et al. Schema Theory: Critical Review and Implications for the Role of Cognition in a New Theory of Motor Learning , 2003, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.
[70] Andrew W. Fitzgibbon,et al. Direct least squares fitting of ellipses , 1996, Proceedings of 13th International Conference on Pattern Recognition.
[71] G. Pezzulo,et al. Interactional leader–follower sensorimotor communication strategies during repetitive joint actions , 2015, Journal of The Royal Society Interface.
[72] Giulio Sandini,et al. Understanding Object Weight from Human and Humanoid Lifting Actions , 2014, IEEE Transactions on Autonomous Mental Development.
[73] R. C. Oldfield. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.
[74] Scott T. Grafton,et al. Virtual lesions of the anterior intraparietal area disrupt goal-dependent on-line adjustments of grasp , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.
[75] Daniel Jokisch,et al. Self recognition versus recognition of others by biological motion: viewpoint-dependent effects. , 2004, Perception.
[76] M. Arbib,et al. Language within our grasp , 1998, Trends in Neurosciences.
[77] M. Candidi,et al. Kinematics fingerprints of leader and follower role-taking during cooperative joint actions , 2013, Experimental Brain Research.
[78] Denise Y. P. Henriques,et al. Visuomotor adaptation and generalization with repeated and varied training , 2013, Experimental Brain Research.
[79] G. Johansson. Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis , 1973 .
[80] 付伶俐. 打磨Using Language,倡导新理念 , 2014 .
[81] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Action recognition in the premotor cortex. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[82] Derek Hoiem,et al. Action Recognition , 2014, Computer Vision, A Reference Guide.
[83] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Activation of human primary motor cortex during action observation: a neuromagnetic study. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[84] L. Fadiga,et al. Sensorimotor communication in professional quartets , 2014, Neuropsychologia.
[85] M. A. Goodale,et al. Factors affecting higher-order movement planning: a kinematic analysis of human prehension , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[86] Janeen D. Loehr,et al. Monitoring Individual and Joint Action Outcomes in Duet Music Performance , 2013, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[87] Matthias Niemeier,et al. A right hemisphere dominance for bimanual grasps , 2012, Experimental Brain Research.
[88] Luca Turella,et al. Mirror neurons in humans: Consisting or confounding evidence? , 2009, Brain and Language.
[89] V. Gallese. The Roots of Empathy: The Shared Manifold Hypothesis and the Neural Basis of Intersubjectivity , 2003, Psychopathology.
[90] M. Jeannerod. The timing of natural prehension movements. , 1984, Journal of motor behavior.
[91] Ravi S. Menon,et al. Visually guided grasping produces fMRI activation in dorsal but not ventral stream brain areas , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.