Episodic specificity induction impacts activity in a core brain network during construction of imagined future experiences

Significance Recent behavioral studies using an episodic specificity induction—training in recollecting details of past experiences—have suggested a role for episodic memory in imagining future events, solving problems, and thinking creatively. The present fMRI study examines the brain regions impacted by the specificity induction. The experiment shows that receiving a specificity induction led to increased activity in key brain regions previously implicated in detailed event construction, including the hippocampus and inferior parietal lobule, when participants imagined future events. These results provide insights into the influence of episodic memory beyond simple remembering, and may help to guide potential applications for individuals from populations characterized by overgeneralized memory and imagination, such as healthy aging and clinical depression. Recent behavioral work suggests that an episodic specificity induction—brief training in recollecting the details of a past experience—enhances performance on subsequent tasks that rely on episodic retrieval, including imagining future experiences, solving open-ended problems, and thinking creatively. Despite these far-reaching behavioral effects, nothing is known about the neural processes impacted by an episodic specificity induction. Related neuroimaging work has linked episodic retrieval with a core network of brain regions that supports imagining future experiences. We tested the hypothesis that key structures in this network are influenced by the specificity induction. Participants received the specificity induction or one of two control inductions and then generated future events and semantic object comparisons during fMRI scanning. After receiving the specificity induction compared with the control, participants exhibited significantly more activity in several core network regions during the construction of imagined events over object comparisons, including the left anterior hippocampus, right inferior parietal lobule, right posterior cingulate cortex, and right ventral precuneus. Induction-related differences in the episodic detail of imagined events significantly modulated induction-related differences in the construction of imagined events in the left anterior hippocampus and right inferior parietal lobule. Resting-state functional connectivity analyses with hippocampal and inferior parietal lobule seed regions and the rest of the brain also revealed significantly stronger core network coupling following the specificity induction compared with the control. These findings provide evidence that an episodic specificity induction selectively targets episodic processes that are commonly linked to key core network regions, including the hippocampus.

[1]  William A. Cunningham,et al.  Type I and Type II error concerns in fMRI research: re-balancing the scale. , 2009, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[2]  Michael D. Rugg,et al.  Retrieval Orientation and the Control of Recollection: An fMRI Study , 2012, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[3]  R. Buckner,et al.  Functional-Anatomic Fractionation of the Brain's Default Network , 2010, Neuron.

[4]  Daniel L. Schacter,et al.  Interactions between Visual Attention and Episodic Retrieval: Dissociable Contributions of Parietal Regions during Gist-Based False Recognition , 2012, Neuron.

[5]  Steen Moeller,et al.  Multiband multislice GE‐EPI at 7 tesla, with 16‐fold acceleration using partial parallel imaging with application to high spatial and temporal whole‐brain fMRI , 2010, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[6]  Troy A. Smith,et al.  Human hippocampus represents space and time during retrieval of real-world memories , 2015, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[7]  B. Biswal,et al.  Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo‐planar mri , 1995, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[8]  A. MacLeod,et al.  Well-being and positive future thinking for the self versus others , 2007 .

[9]  Allan Paivio,et al.  Extensions of the Paivio, Yuille, and Madigan (1968) norms , 2004, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.

[10]  Daniel L. Schacter,et al.  The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Remembering the past and Imagining the Future: Selective Effects of an Episodic Specificity Induction on Detail Generation , 2022 .

[11]  D. Schacter,et al.  Constructive episodic simulation: Temporal distance and detail of past and future events modulate hippocampal engagement , 2008, Hippocampus.

[12]  D. Schacter,et al.  The cognitive neuroscience of constructive memory: remembering the past and imagining the future , 2007, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[13]  R. N. Spreng,et al.  The Future of Memory: Remembering, Imagining, and the Brain , 2012, Neuron.

[14]  Nadine Eberhardt,et al.  Memory Enhancing Techniques For Investigative Interviewing The Cognitive Interview , 2016 .

[15]  N. Ellis,et al.  The specificity of autobiographical memory and imageability of the future , 1996, Memory & cognition.

[16]  H. Eichenbaum Memory on time , 2013, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[17]  Steve Majerus,et al.  The Neural Basis of Temporal Order Processing in Past and Future Thought , 2015, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[18]  Hallvard Røe Evensmoen,et al.  Long-axis specialization of the human hippocampus , 2013, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[19]  Alison R Preston,et al.  Hippocampal contribution to the novel use of relational information in declarative memory , 2004, Hippocampus.

[20]  Daniel L Schacter,et al.  Ventromedial prefrontal cortex supports affective future simulation by integrating distributed knowledge , 2014, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[21]  M. Fox,et al.  The global signal and observed anticorrelated resting state brain networks. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[22]  Daniel L Schacter,et al.  Remembering the past and imagining the future: Identifying and enhancing the contribution of episodic memory , 2016, Memory studies.

[23]  Alana T. Wong,et al.  Remembering the past and imagining the future: Common and distinct neural substrates during event construction and elaboration , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[24]  Alana T. Wong,et al.  Age-Related Changes in the Episodic Simulation of Future Events , 2008, Psychological science.

[25]  D. Schacter,et al.  Age-related neural changes in autobiographical remembering and imagining , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[26]  D. Schacter,et al.  Remembering what could have happened: Neural correlates of episodic counterfactual thinking , 2013, Neuropsychologia.

[27]  Catriona M. Morrison,et al.  Episodic Future Thinking: Linking Neuropsychological Performance with Episodic Detail in Young and Old Adults , 2013, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[28]  Pascale Piolino,et al.  Factors affecting medial temporal lobe engagement for past and future episodic events: An ALE meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies , 2012, Brain and Cognition.

[29]  Marisa O. Hollinshead,et al.  The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. , 2011, Journal of neurophysiology.

[30]  Kevin Murphy,et al.  The impact of global signal regression on resting state correlations: Are anti-correlated networks introduced? , 2009, NeuroImage.

[31]  D. Schacter,et al.  Specifying the core network supporting episodic simulation and episodic memory by activation likelihood estimation , 2015, Neuropsychologia.

[32]  Charan Ranganath,et al.  The hippocampus: a special place for time , 2016, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[33]  D. Schacter,et al.  Hippocampal contributions to the episodic simulation of specific and general future events , 2011, Hippocampus.

[34]  G. Winocur,et al.  Aging and autobiographical memory: dissociating episodic from semantic retrieval. , 2002, Psychology and aging.

[35]  D. Schacter,et al.  The Hippocampus and Imagining the Future: Where Do We Stand? , 2011, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[36]  E. Tulving [Episodic memory: from mind to brain]. , 2004, Revue neurologique.

[37]  T. Dalgleish,et al.  Enhancing Autobiographical Memory Specificity Through Cognitive Training , 2013 .

[38]  Hans Knutsson,et al.  Cluster failure: Why fMRI inferences for spatial extent have inflated false-positive rates , 2016, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[39]  E. Maguire,et al.  Memory , Imagination , and Predicting the Future : A Common Brain Mechanism ? , 2013 .

[40]  W. K. Simmons,et al.  Circular analysis in systems neuroscience: the dangers of double dipping , 2009, Nature Neuroscience.

[41]  D. Schacter,et al.  The Brain's Default Network , 2008, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[42]  Sylvia Hach,et al.  Neural changes associated with the generation of specific past and future events in depression , 2014, Neuropsychologia.

[43]  Olaf Blanke,et al.  Brain system for mental orientation in space, time, and person , 2015, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[44]  M. Corballis,et al.  A role for the hippocampus in encoding simulations of future events , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[45]  Daniel L Schacter,et al.  An episodic specificity induction enhances means-end problem solving in young and older adults. , 2014, Psychology and aging.

[46]  Donna Rose Addis,et al.  The neural correlates of specific versus general autobiographical memory construction and elaboration , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[47]  D. Schacter,et al.  Episodic simulation of past and future events in older adults: Evidence from an experimental recombination task. , 2010, Psychology and aging.

[48]  E. Maguire,et al.  Constructing, Perceiving, and Maintaining Scenes: Hippocampal Activity and Connectivity , 2014, Cerebral cortex.

[49]  Daniel L Schacter,et al.  Worrying about the future: An episodic specificity induction impacts problem solving, reappraisal, and well-being. , 2016, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[50]  D. Schacter,et al.  Remembering the past to imagine the future: the prospective brain , 2007, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[51]  Rebecca Saxe,et al.  Contributions of episodic retrieval and mentalizing to autobiographical thought: Evidence from functional neuroimaging, resting-state connectivity, and fMRI meta-analyses , 2014, NeuroImage.

[52]  Archana Venkataraman,et al.  Intrinsic functional connectivity as a tool for human connectomics: theory, properties, and optimization. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.

[53]  D. Hassabis,et al.  Using Imagination to Understand the Neural Basis of Episodic Memory , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[54]  Daniel L. Schacter,et al.  Autobiographical Planning and the Brain: Activation and Its Modulation by Qualitative Features , 2015, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[55]  Mieke Verfaellie,et al.  Distinct hippocampal regions make unique contributions to relational memory , 2009, Hippocampus.

[56]  Lila Davachi,et al.  How the hippocampus preserves order: the role of prediction and context , 2015, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[57]  Antoine Lutti,et al.  Investigating the functions of subregions within anterior hippocampus , 2015, Cortex.

[58]  H. Pashler,et al.  Puzzlingly High Correlations in fMRI Studies of Emotion, Personality, and Social Cognition 1 , 2009, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[59]  Olaf Blanke,et al.  Self in Time: Imagined Self-Location Influences Neural Activity Related to Mental Time Travel , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[60]  D. Schacter,et al.  Characterizing Age-related Changes in Remembering the past and Imagining the Future Experiment 1 , 2022 .

[61]  Justin L. Vincent,et al.  Evidence for a frontoparietal control system revealed by intrinsic functional connectivity. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[62]  Daniel L Schacter,et al.  Constructive episodic simulation: dissociable effects of a specificity induction on remembering, imagining, and describing in young and older adults. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[63]  Gagan S Wig,et al.  Repetition-related reductions in neural activity reveal component processes of mental simulation. , 2014, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[64]  Jean-Luc Anton,et al.  Region of interest analysis using an SPM toolbox , 2010 .

[65]  R. N. Spreng,et al.  Imagining the future: Evidence for a hippocampal contribution to constructive processing , 2013, Hippocampus.

[66]  J. Polimeni,et al.  Blipped‐controlled aliasing in parallel imaging for simultaneous multislice echo planar imaging with reduced g‐factor penalty , 2012, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[67]  Donna Rose Addis,et al.  Creativity and Memory , 2015, Psychological science.

[68]  M. Conway,et al.  The construction of autobiographical memories in the self-memory system. , 2000, Psychological review.