Sensing with tools extends somatosensory processing beyond the body

[1]  H. Ehrsson,et al.  Sensorimotor predictions and tool use: Hand-held tools attenuate self-touch , 2017, Cognition.

[2]  Benoit P. Delhaye,et al.  Simulating tactile signals from the whole hand with millisecond precision , 2017, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[3]  Kevin N Laland,et al.  Extended spider cognition , 2017, Animal Cognition.

[4]  T. Makin,et al.  Neurocognitive barriers to the embodiment of technology , 2017, Nature Biomedical Engineering.

[5]  Xiaoqin Wang,et al.  Importance of spike timing in touch: an analogy with hearing? , 2016, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[6]  Vincent Hayward,et al.  Spatial patterns of cutaneous vibration during whole-hand haptic interactions , 2016, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[7]  Alessandro Farnè,et al.  Tool-use: An open window into body representation and its plasticity , 2016, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[8]  V. Hayward,et al.  Segregation of Tactile Input Features in Neurons of the Cuneate Nucleus , 2014, Neuron.

[9]  J. A. Pruszynski,et al.  Edge-orientation processing in first-order tactile neurons , 2014, Nature Neuroscience.

[10]  Luke E. Miller,et al.  Tool morphology constrains the effects of tool use on body representations. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[11]  Vincent Hayward,et al.  Spatio-temporal skin strain distributions evoke low variability spike responses in cuneate neurons , 2014, Journal of The Royal Society Interface.

[12]  Dori Derdikman,et al.  Pre-neuronal morphological processing of object location by individual whiskers , 2013, Nature Neuroscience.

[13]  Angelo Arleo,et al.  Integration of Sensory Quanta in Cuneate Nucleus Neurons In Vivo , 2013, PloS one.

[14]  Hannes P. Saal,et al.  Millisecond Precision Spike Timing Shapes Tactile Perception , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[15]  E. Ahissar,et al.  Motor-Sensory Confluence in Tactile Perception , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[16]  Safa Bozkurt Coşkun,et al.  Transverse Vibration Analysis of Euler-Bernoulli Beams Using Analytical Approximate Techniques , 2011 .

[17]  Chih-Jen Lin,et al.  LIBSVM: A library for support vector machines , 2011, TIST.

[18]  P. Haggard,et al.  A supramodal representation of the body surface , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[19]  Michael S. Lewicki,et al.  Learning Efficient Auditory Codes Using Spikes Predicts Cochlear Filters , 2004, NIPS.

[20]  C. Spence,et al.  Extending or projecting peripersonal space with tools? Multisensory interactions highlight only the distal and proximal ends of tools , 2004, Neuroscience Letters.

[21]  Scott H. Johnson-Frey The neural bases of complex tool use in humans , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[22]  R. Johansson,et al.  First spikes in ensembles of human tactile afferents code complex spatial fingertip events , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[23]  A. Yuille,et al.  Object perception as Bayesian inference. , 2004, Annual review of psychology.

[24]  R. W. Young Evolution of the human hand: the role of throwing and clubbing , 2003, Journal of anatomy.

[25]  S. Kitazawa,et al.  Sensation at the tips of invisible tools , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[26]  Kenneth O. Johnson,et al.  The roles and functions of cutaneous mechanoreceptors , 2001, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[27]  Hiroshi Imamizu,et al.  Human cerebellar activity reflecting an acquired internal model of a new tool , 2000, Nature.

[28]  S S Hsiao,et al.  Detection of vibration transmitted through an object grasped in the hand. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[29]  A. Clark,et al.  The Extended Mind , 1998, Analysis.

[30]  Randall D. Beer,et al.  The brain has a body: adaptive behavior emerges from interactions of nervous system, body and environment , 1997, Trends in Neurosciences.

[31]  Corinna Cortes,et al.  Support-Vector Networks , 1995, Machine Learning.

[32]  J. Bell,et al.  The structure and function of pacinian corpuscles: A review , 1994, Progress in Neurobiology.

[33]  M T Turvey,et al.  Perceiving the vertical distances of surfaces by means of a hand-held probe. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[34]  G. Jansson,et al.  The Detection and Localization of Objects by the Blind with the Aid of Long-Cane Tapping Sounds , 1986 .

[35]  G. Gladwell The inverse problem for the vibrating beam , 1984, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. A. Mathematical and Physical Sciences.

[36]  R. Johansson,et al.  Responses of mechanoreceptive afferent units in the glabrous skin of the human hand to sinusoidal skin displacements , 1982, Brain Research.

[37]  A. Iggo,et al.  Correlative physiological and morphological studies of rapidly adapting mechanoreceptors in cat's glabrous skin. , 1977, The Journal of physiology.

[38]  V. Mountcastle,et al.  The sense of flutter-vibration: comparison of the human capacity with response patterns of mechanoreceptive afferents from the monkey hand. , 1968, Journal of neurophysiology.

[39]  John Cohen The World of Touch , 1952, Nature.

[40]  Nora M. Villanueva,et al.  FWDselect: An R Package for Variable Selection in Regression Models , 2016, R J..

[41]  R Core Team,et al.  R: A language and environment for statistical computing. , 2014 .

[42]  S. Kabanikhin Definitions and examples of inverse and ill-posed problems , 2008 .

[43]  Vincent Hayward,et al.  Haptic Shape Cues, Invariants, Priors, and Interface Design , 2008 .

[44]  R. Klatzky,et al.  Tactile roughness perception with a rigid link interposed between skin and surface , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.