Alpha Oscillations Modulate Preparatory Activity in Marmoset Area 8Ad

Cognitive control often requires suppression of prepotent stimulus-driven responses in favor of less potent alternatives. Suppression of prepotent saccades has been shown to require proactive inhibition in the frontoparietal saccade network. Electrophysiological evidence in macaque monkeys has revealed neural correlates of such inhibition in this network; however, the interlaminar instantiation of inhibitory processes remains poorly understood because these areas lie deep within sulci in macaques, rendering them inaccessible to laminar recordings. Here, we addressed this gap by exploiting the mostly lissencephalic cortex of the common marmoset (Callithrix jacchus). We inserted linear electrode arrays into areas 8Ad—the putative marmoset frontal eye field—and the lateral intraparietal area of two male marmosets and recorded neural activity during performance of a task comprised of alternating blocks of trials requiring a saccade either toward a large, high-luminance stimulus or the inhibition of this prepotent response in favor of a saccade toward a small, low-luminance stimulus. We observed prominent task-dependent activity in both alpha/gamma bands of the LFP and discharge rates of single neurons in area 8Ad during a prestimulus task epoch in which the animals had been instructed which of these two tasks to perform but before peripheral stimulus onset. These data are consistent with a model in which rhythmic alpha-band activity in deeper layers inhibits spiking in upper layers to support proactive inhibitory saccade control. SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Failures to inhibit automatic saccadic responses are a hallmark of many neuropsychiatric disorders, but how this process is implemented across the cortical layers in the frontoparietal saccade network remains unknown because many of the areas are inaccessible to laminar recordings in macaques. Here, we investigated laminar neural activity in marmoset monkeys, which have a smooth cortex. Monkeys were required either to generate or inhibit a prepotent saccade response. In area 8Ad, the putative frontal eye field in marmosets, rhythmic alpha-band activity (9–14 Hz) was higher in deeper layers and spiking activity was lower in upper layers when the animals were instructed to suppress a saccade toward a peripheral stimulus. Reduced alpha power during task preparation may be the underlying common neural basis of a saccade suppression deficit.

[1]  D. Levy,et al.  Functional neuroanatomy of antisaccade eye movements investigated with positron emission tomography. , 1995, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[2]  Shawn R. Olsen,et al.  Gain control by layer six in cortical circuits of vision , 2012, Nature.

[3]  G. Pfurtscheller,et al.  Event-related synchronization (ERS) in the alpha band--an electrophysiological correlate of cortical idling: a review. , 1996, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[4]  K. Johnston,et al.  An Approach to Understanding the Neural Circuitry of Saccade Control in the Cerebral Cortex Using Antidromic Identification in the Awake Behaving Macaque Monkey Model , 2011 .

[5]  Christine M Constantinople,et al.  Deep Cortical Layers Are Activated Directly by Thalamus , 2013, Science.

[6]  Jun Kunimatsu,et al.  Roles of the Primate Motor Thalamus in the Generation of Antisaccades , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[7]  Earl K. Miller,et al.  Laminar recordings in frontal cortex suggest distinct layers for maintenance and control of working memory , 2018, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[8]  C. Bruce,et al.  Primate frontal eye fields. II. Physiological and anatomical correlates of electrically evoked eye movements. , 1985, Journal of neurophysiology.

[9]  D P Munoz,et al.  Role of Primate Superior Colliculus in Preparation and Execution of Anti-Saccades and Pro-Saccades , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[10]  Ole Jensen,et al.  Gamma Activity Coupled to Alpha Phase as a Mechanism for Top-Down Controlled Gating , 2015, PloS one.

[11]  Stefan Everling,et al.  Methods for chair restraint and training of the common marmoset on oculomotor tasks. , 2018, Journal of neurophysiology.

[12]  Michael Petrides,et al.  The marmoset brain in stereotaxic coordinates , 2012 .

[13]  J. Movshon,et al.  The statistical reliability of signals in single neurons in cat and monkey visual cortex , 1983, Vision Research.

[14]  David A. Leopold,et al.  Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience Systems Neuroscience , 2022 .

[15]  C. Koch,et al.  Is perception discrete or continuous? , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[16]  Stefan Everling,et al.  Frontoparietal activation with preparation for antisaccades. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[17]  Ravi S. Menon,et al.  Human fMRI evidence for the neural correlates of preparatory set , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[18]  Ole Jensen,et al.  A biologically plausible mechanism for neuronal coding organized by the phase of alpha oscillations , 2016, The European journal of neuroscience.

[19]  H. Berger Über das Elektrenkephalogramm des Menschen , 1938, Archiv für Psychiatrie und Nervenkrankheiten.

[20]  D. Manoach,et al.  Cortical Neurodynamics of Inhibitory Control , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[21]  P. Roelfsema,et al.  Alpha and gamma oscillations characterize feedback and feedforward processing in monkey visual cortex , 2014, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[22]  Jay A. Edelman,et al.  Antisaccade velocity, but not latency, results from a lack of saccade visual guidance , 2006, Vision Research.

[23]  Á. Pascual-Leone,et al.  α-Band Electroencephalographic Activity over Occipital Cortex Indexes Visuospatial Attention Bias and Predicts Visual Target Detection , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[24]  T. Ergenoğlu,et al.  Alpha rhythm of the EEG modulates visual detection performance in humans. , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[25]  E. John,et al.  Perceptual framing and cortical alpha rhythm , 1981, Neuropsychologia.

[26]  L Elliot Hong,et al.  High vs low frequency neural oscillations in schizophrenia. , 2011, Schizophrenia bulletin.

[27]  Jordan P. Hamm,et al.  Alpha oscillations and the control of voluntary saccadic behavior , 2012, Experimental Brain Research.

[28]  E. Miller,et al.  An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. , 2001, Annual review of neuroscience.

[29]  Ravi S. Menon,et al.  Preparatory set associated with pro-saccades and anti-saccades in humans investigated with event-related FMRI. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[30]  D. Munoz,et al.  The influence of stimulus direction and eccentricity on pro- and anti-saccades in humans , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[31]  R. Desimone,et al.  Laminar differences in gamma and alpha coherence in the ventral stream , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[32]  C. Curtis,et al.  Success and Failure Suppressing Reflexive Behavior , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[33]  R. VanRullen,et al.  The Phase of Ongoing EEG Oscillations Predicts Visual Perception , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[34]  D. Munoz,et al.  Look away: the anti-saccade task and the voluntary control of eye movement , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[35]  Beatriz Luna,et al.  Frontal preparatory neural oscillations associated with cognitive control: A developmental study comparing young adults and adolescents , 2016, NeuroImage.

[36]  D. Munoz,et al.  Reflex suppression in the anti-saccade task is dependent on prestimulus neural processes. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[37]  L. Pinneo On noise in the nervous system. , 1966, Psychological review.

[38]  Mariya V. Cherkasova,et al.  Antisaccades and Task Switching , 2002 .

[39]  D. Leopold,et al.  Layer-Specific Entrainment of Gamma-Band Neural Activity by the Alpha Rhythm in Monkey Visual Cortex , 2012, Current Biology.

[40]  Tai Sing Lee,et al.  Local field potentials indicate network state and account for neuronal response variability , 2010, Journal of Computational Neuroscience.

[41]  Ravi S. Menon,et al.  Frontoparietal Functional Connectivity in the Common Marmoset , 2016, Cerebral cortex.

[42]  Philipp Berens,et al.  CircStat: AMATLABToolbox for Circular Statistics , 2009, Journal of Statistical Software.

[43]  Christopher Kennard,et al.  An internationally standardised antisaccade protocol , 2013, Vision Research.

[44]  O. Jensen,et al.  Shaping Functional Architecture by Oscillatory Alpha Activity: Gating by Inhibition , 2010, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[45]  S. Everling,et al.  The antisaccade: a review of basic research and clinical studies , 1998, Neuropsychologia.

[46]  R. Oostenveld,et al.  Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG- and MEG-data , 2007, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[47]  Eduardo A. Aponte,et al.  Inhibition failures and late errors in the antisaccade task: influence of cue delay. , 2018, Journal of neurophysiology.

[48]  H. Eichenbaum,et al.  Measuring phase-amplitude coupling between neuronal oscillations of different frequencies. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.

[49]  Stefan Everling,et al.  Theta and beta synchrony coordinate frontal eye fields and anterior cingulate cortex during sensorimotor mapping , 2017, Nature Communications.

[50]  David A. Leopold,et al.  Brains, Genes, and Primates , 2015, Neuron.

[51]  S. Hutton,et al.  The antisaccade task as a research tool in psychopathology: a critical review. , 2006, Psychophysiology.

[52]  B. Connors,et al.  Intrinsic oscillations of neocortex generated by layer 5 pyramidal neurons. , 1991, Science.

[53]  D P Munoz,et al.  Neuronal Correlates for Preparatory Set Associated with Pro-Saccades and Anti-Saccades in the Primate Frontal Eye Field , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[54]  David A. Leopold,et al.  The marmoset monkey as a model for visual neuroscience , 2015, Neuroscience Research.

[55]  Kara A. Dyckman,et al.  Pre-Cue Fronto-Occipital Alpha Phase and Distributed Cortical Oscillations Predict Failures of Cognitive Control , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[56]  C. Bruce,et al.  Primate frontal eye fields. I. Single neurons discharging before saccades. , 1985, Journal of neurophysiology.

[57]  H. Okano,et al.  Generation of transgenic non-human primates with germline transmission , 2009, Nature.

[58]  T. Womelsdorf,et al.  Theta–gamma coordination between anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex indexes correct attention shifts , 2015, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[59]  John H. Reynolds,et al.  Active Vision in Marmosets: A Model System for Visual Neuroscience , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[60]  R. Romo,et al.  α-Oscillations in the monkey sensorimotor network influence discrimination performance by rhythmical inhibition of neuronal spiking , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[61]  Mariya V. Cherkasova,et al.  Antisaccades and task switching: studies of control processes in saccadic function in normal subjects and schizophrenic patients. , 2002, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[62]  D. Munoz,et al.  Influence of stimulus eccentricity and direction on characteristics of pro- and antisaccades in non-human primates. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[63]  Christopher W. Pleydell-Pearce,et al.  The phase of pre-stimulus alpha oscillations influences the visual perception of stimulus timing , 2016, NeuroImage.