Electrophysiological correlates of common-onset visual masking

In common-onset visual masking (COVM) the target and the mask come into view simultaneously. Masking occurs when the mask remains on the screen for longer after deletion of the target. Enns and Di Lollo [Enns, J. T., & Di Lollo, V. (2000). What's new in visual masking? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(9), 345-352] have argued that this type of masking can be explained by re-entrant visual processing. In the present studies we used high-density event-related brain potentials (HD-ERP) to obtain neural evidence for re-entrant processing in COVM. In two experiments the participants' task was to indicate the presence or absence of a vertical bar situated at the lower part of a ring highlighted by the mask. The only difference between the experiments was the duration of the target: 13 and 40 ms for the first and second experiment respectively. Behavioral results were consistent between experiments: COVM was stronger as a joint function of a large set size and longer trailing mask duration. Electrophysiological data from both studies revealed modulation of a posterior P2 component around 220 ms post-stimulus onset associated with masking. Further, in the critical experimental condition we revealed a significant relation between the amplitude of the P2 and behavioural response accuracy. We hypothesize that this re-activation of early visual areas reflects re-entrant feedback from higher to lower visual areas, providing converging evidence for re-entrance as an explanation for COVM.

[1]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  Handbook Of Psychophysiology , 2019 .

[2]  Christian Keysers,et al.  Visual masking and RSVP reveal neural competition , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[3]  Richard B Buxton,et al.  Putting spatial attention on the map: timing and localization of stimulus selection processes in striate and extrastriate visual areas , 2001, Vision Research.

[4]  J. Enns,et al.  What’s new in visual masking? , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[5]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Involvement of striate and extrastriate visual cortical areas in spatial attention , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[6]  Kara D. Federmeier,et al.  Handbook of Psychophysiology: Event-Related Brain Potentials: Methods, Theory, and Applications , 2007 .

[7]  D. M. Green,et al.  Signal detection theory and psychophysics , 1966 .

[8]  Á. Pascual-Leone,et al.  Fast Backprojections from the Motion to the Primary Visual Area Necessary for Visual Awareness , 2001, Science.

[9]  Ronald A. Rensink,et al.  Object substitution without reentry , 2002 .

[10]  Frouke Hermens,et al.  Comment on "Competition for consciousness among visual events: the psychophysics of reentrant visual processes" (Di Lollo, Enns, & Rensink, 2000). , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[11]  A. Treisman,et al.  Search asymmetry: a diagnostic for preattentive processing of separable features. , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[12]  Arnulf Remole,et al.  VISUAL MASKING: AN INTEGRATIVE APPROACH , 1985 .

[13]  M I Posner,et al.  Topography of the N400: brain electrical activity reflecting semantic expectancy. , 1993, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[14]  Ronald A. Rensink,et al.  Competition for consciousness among visual events: the psychophysics of reentrant visual processes. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[15]  R. Martín,et al.  Application of the zinc iodide-osmium tetroxide impregnation of synaptic vesicles in cephalopod nerves. , 1969, Brain research.

[16]  W. Cowan,et al.  Annual Review of Neuroscience , 1995 .

[17]  V. Lamme,et al.  The distinct modes of vision offered by feedforward and recurrent processing , 2000, Trends in Neurosciences.

[18]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Delayed Striate Cortical Activation during Spatial Attention , 2002, Neuron.

[19]  J. Fermaglich Electric Fields of the Brain: The Neurophysics of EEG , 1982 .

[20]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  Handbook of psychophysiology (2nd ed.). , 2000 .

[21]  J. M. Hupé,et al.  Cortical feedback improves discrimination between figure and background by V1, V2 and V3 neurons , 1998, Nature.

[22]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. , 1995, Annual review of neuroscience.

[23]  Olivier Pascalis,et al.  Specialization of Neural Mechanisms Underlying Face Recognition in Human Infants , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[24]  Michael W. Spratling,et al.  A Feedback Model of Visual Attention , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[25]  Keith A Hutchison,et al.  Masking by object substitution: dissociation of masking and cuing effects. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[26]  Nadim Joni Shah,et al.  The Neural Basis of Perceptual Hypothesis Generation and Testing , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[27]  F. Perrin,et al.  Spherical splines for scalp potential and current density mapping. , 1989, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[28]  D. Tucker Spatial sampling of head electrical fields: the geodesic sensor net. , 1993, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[29]  Miguel A. L. Nicolelis,et al.  Advances in neural population coding , 2001 .

[30]  J. Enns,et al.  Object Substitution: A New Form of Masking in Unattended Visual Locations , 1997 .

[31]  T Allison,et al.  Contextual guidance of attention: human intracranial event-related potential evidence for feedback modulation in anatomically early temporally late stages of visual processing. , 2001, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[32]  D. Mumford,et al.  The role of the primary visual cortex in higher level vision , 1998, Vision Research.

[33]  B. Breitmeyer,et al.  Recent models and findings in visual backward masking: A comparison, review, and update , 2000, Perception & psychophysics.