DELAYED REWARD AND COST DISCOUNTING

28) chose between smaller, immediate and larger, delayed hypothetical monetary amounts in a cost and a reward condition. For both conditions, the choice procedure yielded equivalence points that measure the immediate amount reward or cost that is subjectively equivalent to the larger delayed reward or cost. Equivalence pOints from each of eight delays were used to estimate a discounting parameter for both hyperbolic and exponential discount functions. Hyperbolic functions accounted for more of the variance in both the reward and the cost condition, and delayed reward and cost discounting was significantly correlated. Participants discounted the value of delayed rewards to a greater degree than delayed costs. Previous research has found that substance abusers discount delayed rewards more than controls. Implications of the present findings for substance abuse and other health behaviors are discussed. People routinely choose between acts, the consequences of which may be positive and negative and immediate and delayed. For example, one may choose to turn off the alarm in the morning and sleep an extra 30 minutes. This would produce a relatively higher valued immediate outcome, but a relatively lower valued delayed outcome when one is late for work. Or, one could attend college rather than seek employment after high school. This would produce a relatively lower valued, lower income short-term future, but a relatively higher valued, higher income long-term future. Understanding how we choose between sooner and later rewards and costs has been an important task in psychological and economic research for some time (e.g., Ainslie, 1975; Kagel, Battalio, & Green, 1995; Mischel & Grusec, 1967; Rachlin, 1974). A key issue in understanding these choice situations has been describing the manner in which the value of rewards and costs varies as a function of delay (Kirby, 1997; Kirby

[1]  R. Thaler Some empirical evidence on dynamic inconsistency , 1981 .

[2]  J. Kagel,et al.  Economic Choice Theory: An Experimental Analysis of Animal Behavior , 1995 .

[3]  Amy L Odum,et al.  Discounting of delayed health gains and losses by current, never- and ex-smokers of cigarettes. , 2002, Nicotine & tobacco research : official journal of the Society for Research on Nicotine and Tobacco.

[4]  R. Vuchinich,et al.  Temporal changes in the value of objects of choice: Discounting, behavior patterns, and health behavior. , 2000 .

[5]  W K Bickel,et al.  Impulsive and self-control choices in opioid-dependent patients and non-drug-using control participants: drug and monetary rewards. , 1997, Experimental and clinical psychopharmacology.

[6]  D. Cross,et al.  Subjective probability and delay. , 1991, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[7]  G. Loewenstein Frames of mind in intertemporal choice , 1988 .

[8]  M. Z. Deluty,et al.  Self-control and impulsiveness involving aversive events. , 1978 .

[9]  R. Herrnstein,et al.  Preference Reversals Due to Myopic Discounting of Delayed Reward , 1995 .

[10]  H Rachlin,et al.  Commitment, choice and self-control. , 1972, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[11]  H. Rachlin Four teleological theories of addiction , 1997 .

[12]  W. Mischel,et al.  Effects of expected delay time on the subjective value of rewards and punishments. , 1969, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[13]  L. Green,et al.  Discounting of delayed rewards: Models of individual choice. , 1995, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[14]  W. Mischel,et al.  Waiting for rewards and punishments: effects of time and probability on choice. , 1967, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[15]  Marjorie K. Shelley Outcome signs, question frames and discount rates , 1993 .

[16]  G. Ainslie Specious reward: a behavioral theory of impulsiveness and impulse control. , 1975, Psychological bulletin.

[17]  L. Green,et al.  Discounting of Delayed Rewards: A Life-Span Comparison , 1994 .

[18]  W. G. Whitehouse,et al.  SELF-CONTROL AND COMMITMENT INVOLVING AVERSIVE EVENTS , 1983 .

[19]  J. Frank Yates,et al.  Preferences for Deferred Losses , 1975 .

[20]  S. Mitchell,et al.  Measures of impulsivity in cigarette smokers and non-smokers , 1999, Psychopharmacology.

[21]  A. Odum,et al.  Impulsivity and cigarette smoking: delay discounting in current, never, and ex-smokers , 1999, Psychopharmacology.

[22]  R. Vuchinich,et al.  Reliability of a Measure of Temporal Discounting , 2000 .

[23]  A. Gallant,et al.  Nonlinear Statistical Models , 1988 .

[24]  S. Mobini,et al.  Theory and method in the quantitative analysis of ”impulsive choice” behaviour: implications for psychopharmacology , 1999, Psychopharmacology.

[25]  R. Vuchinich,et al.  Hyperbolic temporal discounting in social drinkers and problem drinkers. , 1998, Experimental and clinical psychopharmacology.

[26]  A. Rapoport,et al.  Discount rates inferred from decisions: an experimental study , 1987 .

[27]  U. Ben-Zion,et al.  Discount rates inferred from decisions: an experimental study , 1989 .