Transitions Between Low and High Levels of Mental Workload can Improve Multitasking Performance

OCCUPATIONAL APPLICATIONS Complex and dynamic environments including military operations, healthcare, aviation, and driving require operators to transition seamlessly between levels of mental workload. However, little is known about how the rate of an increase in workload impacts multitasking performance, especially in the context of real-world tasks. We evaluated both gradual and sudden workload increases in the dynamic multitasking environment of an Unmanned Aerial Vehicle (UAV) command and control testbed and compared them to constant workload. Workload transitions were found to improve response time and accuracy compared to when workload was held constant at low or high. These results suggest that workload transitions may allow operators to better regulate mental resources. These findings can also inform the design of operations and technology to assist operators’ management of cognitive resources, which include negating the adverse effects of vigilance decrements during low workload periods and data overload during high workload periods. TECHNICAL ABSTRACT Background High workload and workload transitions can affect performance; however, it is not clear how the rate of transition from low to high workload influences performance in a multitasking setting. Purpose We investigated the effect of workload transition rate on performance in a multitasking environment that is akin to the expectations of operators in complex, data-rich work domains. Method An Unmanned Aerial Vehicle (UAV) command and control testbed was used to vary workload between low, high, gradually transitioning from low to high, and suddenly transitioning from low to high. Performance measures consisted of the response time and accuracy of one primary task and three secondary tasks. Analyses compared: (a) performance differences between gradual and sudden increases in workload; (b) performance during the low workload phases of the workload transitions; and (c) performance during the high workload phases of the workload transitions. Results Overall, there were limited performance differences between gradual and sudden workload transitions. However, both types of transitions led to better performance than constant workload, lending some support for the effort regulation explanation which suggests that participants actively evaluated the amount of mental resources necessary to successfully complete a task after a workload transition. Conclusions Gradual and sudden workload transitions benefit primary and secondary task performance, suggesting that the applicability of existing theoretical explanations depend on the context. For example, varying task demands can be a means to assist operators in the appropriate regulation of mental resources in domains with interdependent tasks. These findings can inform occupation and technology design to support task management.

[1]  Amanda D. Angie,et al.  The effects of workload history on dual task performance , 2005 .

[2]  G. R. J. Hockey A State Control Theory of Adaptation and Individual Differences in Stress Management , 1986 .

[3]  J. Hittner,et al.  Effects of neuroticism and workload history on performance , 2004 .

[4]  Lixiao Huang,et al.  The Impact of Increasing Autonomy on Training Requirements in a UAV Supervisory Control Task , 2019, Journal of Cognitive Engineering and Decision Making.

[5]  James C. Christensen,et al.  The Effects of Workload Transitions in a Multitasking Environment , 2014 .

[6]  Brian W. Moroney,et al.  Effects of Demand Transitions on Vigilance Performance and Perceived Workload , 1995 .

[7]  Chang S. Nam,et al.  Neural Correlates of Workload Transition in Multitasking: An ACT-R Model of Hysteresis Effect , 2019, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[8]  James L. Szalma,et al.  The Vigilance Decrement Reflects Limitations in Effortful Attention, Not Mindlessness , 2003, Hum. Factors.

[9]  Mara Tanelli,et al.  Guest Editorial: Multifaceted Driver-Vehicle Systems: Toward More Effective Driving Simulations, Reliable Driver Modeling, and Increased Trust and Safety , 2018, IEEE Trans. Hum. Mach. Syst..

[10]  Joel S. Warm,et al.  Demand Transitions and Tracking Performance Efficiency: Structural and Strategic Models , 2005 .

[11]  Cheryl K. Beeler,et al.  Workload history effects: A comparison of sudden increases and decreases on performance , 2006 .

[12]  W. Helton,et al.  Brief mental breaks and content-free cues may not keep you focused , 2012, Experimental Brain Research.

[13]  Amanda D. Angie,et al.  Extraversion and task performance: A fresh look through the workload history lens , 2006 .

[14]  R R Rosa,et al.  Demand transitions and sustained attention. , 1993, The Journal of general psychology.

[15]  H. Gr Compensatory control in the regulation of human performance under stress and high workload; a cognitive-energetical framework. , 1997 .

[16]  Jacob W. Crandall,et al.  Predicting Operator Capacity for Supervisory Control of Multiple UAVs , 2007, Innovations in Intelligent Machines.

[17]  Huib de Ridder,et al.  Hysteresis in Mental Workload and Task Performance , 2016, Hum. Factors.

[18]  Supreeta G. Amin A study to determine the influence of workload on nursing personnel , 2011 .

[19]  Avi Parush,et al.  The Impact of Sudden Events and Spatial Configuration on the Benefits of Prior Information to Situation Awareness and Performance , 2013 .

[20]  Daniël Lakens,et al.  Calculating and reporting effect sizes to facilitate cumulative science: a practical primer for t-tests and ANOVAs , 2013, Front. Psychol..

[21]  Luz-Eugenia Cox-Fuenzalida,et al.  Effect of Workload History on Task Performance , 2007, Hum. Factors.

[22]  Jelle Saldien,et al.  Understanding mental workload: from a clarifying concept analysis toward an implementable framework , 2018, Cognition, Technology & Work.

[23]  M. Scerbo,et al.  Changes in stress and subjective workload over time following a workload transition , 2015 .

[24]  Sara L. Riggs,et al.  Effects of Workload and Workload Transitions on Attention Allocation in a Dual-Task Environment: Evidence From Eye Tracking Metrics , 2020 .

[25]  David W. Biers,et al.  In Search of Workload Context Effects Using a Compensatory Tracking Task , 2005 .

[26]  Tyler H. Shaw,et al.  Effects of warned and unwarned demand transitions on vigilance performance and stress , 2008, Anxiety, stress, and coping.

[27]  Joel S. Warm,et al.  Vigilance Requires Hard Mental Work and Is Stressful , 2008, Hum. Factors.

[28]  Christopher D. Wickens,et al.  Discrete task switching in overload: A meta-analyses and a model , 2015, Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud..

[29]  P A Hancock,et al.  Influence of task demand characteristics on workload and performance. , 1995, The International journal of aviation psychology.

[30]  Peter A. Hancock,et al.  The Effect of Prior Task Loading on Mental Workload , 2011, Hum. Factors.

[31]  Kevin W Williams,et al.  Human Factors Implications of Unmanned Aircraft Accidents: Flight-Control Problems , 2006 .

[32]  Robert A Goldberg,et al.  Memory overload or expectancy effect? 'Hysteresis' revisited. , 1980, Ergonomics.

[33]  J. Warm,et al.  Effects of shifts in the rate of repetitive stimulation on sustained attention , 1975 .

[34]  R. Cumming Human information processing under varying task demand. , 1973, Ergonomics.

[35]  M L Matthews The Influence of Visual Workload History on Visual Performance , 1986, Human factors.

[36]  S. Hart,et al.  Development of NASA-TLX (Task Load Index): Results of Empirical and Theoretical Research , 1988 .

[37]  Tamsyn Edwards,et al.  Factor interaction influences on human performance in air traffic control: the need for a multifactorial model. , 2012, Work.

[38]  G. Matthews,et al.  Task-induced fatigue states and simulated driving performance , 2002, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[39]  L. Fagerström,et al.  Nurses' Experiences of Nonpatient Factors That Affect Nursing Workload: A Study of the PAONCIL Instrument's Nonpatient Factors , 2014, Nursing research and practice.

[40]  P. Hancock,et al.  A Dynamic Model of Stress and Sustained Attention , 1989, Human factors.