Does prosody make the difference? A meta-analysis on relations between prosodic aspects of infant-directed speech and infant outcomes.

Abstract Infant-directed speech (IDS) is the particular voice register observed in the majority of parents in interaction with their infants and differs from natural speech used in conversations with adults by showing exaggerated prosodic features. These prosodic features are supposed to have effects on regulating infant arousal and attention, fostering infant pre-linguistic and linguistic competences and enhancing the expression of positive affect. The present set of meta-analyses was conducted to test these associations and the role of moderators during the first two years of infant life. The results confirmed an overall association between IDS prosody and infant outcomes with prosodic values typical of IDS associated with better outcomes. This association was confirmed for attentional, pre-linguistic and linguistic outcomes with a greater effect on pre-linguistic than linguistic outcomes. An insufficient number of studies was found to test the association with infant emotion expression. Many limitations in the existing body of literature were found, such as a lack of empirical papers exploring IDS prosody in relation to infant outcomes using natural observations. The results and limitations were discussed in light of the necessity to examine the interplay between the quality of IDS prosody and other aspects of parental communicative and caregiving competences. To do so, the contribution of scholars from different fields is needed with the aim to fully understand the multidimensional determinants and influential mechanisms of IDS.

[1]  Herbert L. Pick Comparative and Developmental Approaches to Spatial Cognition , 1983 .

[2]  M. Chetouani,et al.  Motherese in Interaction: At the Cross-Road of Emotion and Cognition? (A Systematic Review) , 2013, PloS one.

[3]  S Kiritani,et al.  Developmental change in auditory preferences for speech stimuli in Japanese infants. , 2001, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[4]  E. Hoff The specificity of environmental influence: socioeconomic status affects early vocabulary development via maternal speech. , 2003, Child development.

[5]  C. Kitamura,et al.  The shift in infant preferences for vowel duration and pitch contour between 6 and 10 months of age. , 2009, Developmental science.

[6]  Patricia K. Kuhl,et al.  Fast Track , 2020, Electric and Hybrid Vehicle Technology International.

[7]  J. Shepherd,et al.  The melody of surprise: maternal surprise vocalizations during play with her infant , 2002 .

[8]  L. Hedges,et al.  Introduction to Meta‐Analysis , 2009, International Coaching Psychology Review.

[9]  A. Fernald Approval and disapproval: infant responsiveness to vocal affect in familiar and unfamiliar languages. , 1993, Child development.

[10]  L. D’Odorico,et al.  Prosodic and lexical aspects of maternal linguistic input to late-talking toddlers. , 2006, International journal of language & communication disorders.

[11]  P. Kaplan,et al.  Infant-directed speech produced by fathers with symptoms of depression: effects on infant associative learning in a conditioned-attention paradigm. , 2007, Infant behavior & development.

[12]  Maria Uther,et al.  Do you speak E-NG-L-I-SH? A comparison of foreigner- and infant-directed speech , 2007, Speech Commun..

[13]  Elena E. Lyakso,et al.  Infant Vocalizations at the First Year of Life Predict Speech Development at 2 - 7 Years: Longitudinal Study , 2014 .

[14]  Adena Schachner,et al.  Infant-directed speech drives social preferences in 5-month-old infants. , 2011, Developmental psychology.

[15]  Shozo Kojima,et al.  Cerebral responses to infant-directed speech and the effect of talker familiarity , 2012, NeuroImage.

[16]  A. Fernald,et al.  Intonation and communicative intent in mothers' speech to infants: is the melody the message? , 1989, Child development.

[17]  S Duval,et al.  Trim and Fill: A Simple Funnel‐Plot–Based Method of Testing and Adjusting for Publication Bias in Meta‐Analysis , 2000, Biometrics.

[18]  J. Cohn,et al.  A combination of vocal fo dynamic and summary features discriminates between three pragmatic categories of infant-directed speech. , 1996, Child development.

[19]  H. Gleitman,et al.  Mother, Id rather do it myself: Some effects and non-effects of maternal speech style , 1977 .

[20]  A. Arteche,et al.  Sadness in mothers' 'baby-talk' predicts affective disorder in adolescent offspring. , 2010, Infant behavior & development.

[21]  Rochelle S. Newman,et al.  Changes in Preference for Infant-Directed Speech in Low and Moderate Noise by 4.5- to 13-Month-Olds. , 2006, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[22]  Robert Rosenthal,et al.  WRITING META-ANALYTIC REVIEWS , 1995 .

[23]  Katsuko Niwano,et al.  Intonation Contour of Japanese Maternal Infant-Directed Speech and Infant Vocal Response , 2002 .

[24]  D. Burnham,et al.  What's New, Pussycat? On Talking to Babies and Animals , 2002, Science.

[25]  J. Bachorowski,et al.  Role of Clinical Diagnosis and Medication Use in Effects of Maternal Depression on Infant-Directed Speech. , 2001, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[26]  N. Berthier,et al.  Preference for infant-directed speech in preterm infants. , 2014, Infant behavior & development.

[27]  H. Papoušek,et al.  The meanings of melodies in motherese in tone and stress languages , 1991 .

[28]  N. Masataka,et al.  Pitch characteristics of Japanese maternal speech to infants , 1992, Journal of Child Language.

[29]  P. Kaplan,et al.  Maternal Sensitivity and the Learning-Promoting Effects of Depressed and Non-Depressed Mothers' Infant-Directed Speech. , 2009, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[30]  A. Tagini,et al.  Linguistic and prosodic aspects of child-directed speech: The role of maternal child-rearing experiences , 2016 .

[31]  B. Tabachnick,et al.  Using Multivariate Statistics , 1983 .

[32]  Catharine H. Echols,et al.  Stressed and word-final syllables in infant-directed speech , 1996 .

[33]  H. Papoušek,et al.  Forms and functions of vocal matching in interactions between mothers and their precanonical infants , 1989 .

[34]  Laura L Namy,et al.  Say It Like You Mean It: Mothers’ Use of Prosody to Convey Word Meaning , 2012, Language and speech.

[35]  M. Goldstein,et al.  Habituation, sensitization, and infants' responses to motherese speech. , 1995, Developmental psychobiology.

[36]  N. Masataka,et al.  Early ontogeny of vocal behavior of Japanese infants in response to maternal speech. , 1992, Child development.

[37]  R. Aslin,et al.  Developmental differences in infant attention to the spectral properties of infant-directed speech. , 1994, Child development.

[38]  D. Stern,et al.  Intonation contours as signals in maternal speech to prelinguistic infants. , 1982 .

[39]  Klaus R. Scherer,et al.  Vocal communication of emotion: A review of research paradigms , 2003, Speech Commun..

[40]  C. A. Ferguson,et al.  Talking to Children , 1977 .

[41]  D. Moore,et al.  Categorization of Infant-Directed Speech , 2002 .

[42]  A. Fernald Four-Month-Old Infants Prefer to Listen to Motherese" , 1985 .

[43]  D. Burnham,et al.  Pitch and Communicative Intent in Mother's Speech: Adjustments for Age and Sex in the First Year , 2003 .

[44]  Mohamed Chetouani,et al.  Do Parentese Prosody and Fathers' Involvement in Interacting Facilitate Social Interaction in Infants Who Later Develop Autism? , 2013, PloS one.

[45]  L. Trainor,et al.  Frontal brain electrical activity (EEG) and heart rate in response to affective infant-directed (ID) speech in 9-month-old infants , 2007, Brain and Cognition.

[46]  James L. Morgan,et al.  Is early word-form processing stress-full? How natural variability supports recognition , 2010, Cognitive Psychology.

[47]  Alejandrina Cristià,et al.  Input to Language: The Phonetics and Perception of Infant-Directed Speech , 2013, Lang. Linguistics Compass.

[48]  L. Trainor,et al.  Is Infant-Directed Speech Prosody a Result of the Vocal Expression of Emotion? , 2000, Psychological science.

[49]  Kathy Hirsh-Pasek,et al.  Word Learning in Infant- and Adult-Directed Speech , 2011, Language learning and development : the official journal of the Society for Language Development.

[50]  C. A. Ferguson,et al.  Talking to Children: Language Input and Acquisition , 1979 .

[51]  M. Gratier,et al.  Imitation and repetition of prosodic contour in vocal interaction at 3 months. , 2011, Developmental psychology.

[52]  J. Culling,et al.  Parental Speech at 6 Months Predicts Joint Attention at 12 Months , 2013 .

[53]  J. Bachorowski,et al.  Child-directed speech produced by mothers with symptoms of depression fails to promote associative learning in 4-month-old infants. , 1999, Child development.

[54]  T. Walker,et al.  The Effects Of Parental Behaviors And Prosody On The Language And Cognitive Development Of Infants , 2012 .

[55]  M. Soderstrom,et al.  Entrainment of prosody in the interaction of mothers with their young children* , 2015, Journal of Child Language.

[56]  M. Rowe,et al.  Child-directed speech: relation to socioeconomic status, knowledge of child development and child vocabulary skill* , 2008, Journal of Child Language.

[57]  J. Morgan,et al.  Acoustical cues and grammatical units in speech to two preverbal infants* , 2008, Journal of Child Language.

[58]  Kathy Hirsh-Pasek,et al.  (Baby)Talk to Me , 2015 .

[59]  L. Murray,et al.  The effects of maternal depression on the 'musicality' of infant-directed speech and conversational engagement , 2010 .

[60]  Kuniaki Sugai,et al.  Maternal Accommodation in Infant-Directed Speech during Mother's and Twin-Infants' Vocal Interactions , 2003, Psychological reports.

[61]  C. A. Ferguson Baby Talk in Six Languages , 1964 .

[62]  M. Soderstrom,et al.  Beyond babytalk: Re-evaluating the nature and content of speech input to preverbal infants , 2007 .

[63]  B. Bettes,et al.  Maternal depression and motherese: temporal and intonational features. , 1988, Child development.

[64]  P. Kuhl,et al.  Acoustic determinants of infant preference for motherese speech , 1987 .

[65]  N. Zaretskaya,et al.  Introspection, attention or awareness? The role of the frontal lobe in binocular rivalry , 2014, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[66]  J. Bachorowski,et al.  Depression Diagnoses and Fundamental Frequency-Based Acoustic Cues in Maternal Infant-Directed Speech , 2014, Language learning and development : the official journal of the Society for Language Development.

[67]  Mohamed Chetouani,et al.  Course of maternal prosodic incitation (motherese) during early development in autism: An exploratory home movie study , 2013 .

[68]  Monja A. Knoll,et al.  Characterising F(0) contour shape in infant- and foreigner-directed speech , 2015, Speech Commun..

[69]  P. Kuhl,et al.  Maternal speech to infants in a tonal language: Support for universal prosodic features in motherese. , 1988 .

[70]  G. Csibra,et al.  Gaze Following in Human Infants Depends on Communicative Signals , 2008, Current Biology.

[71]  Michael C. Frank,et al.  Contributions of Prosodic and Distributional Features of Caregivers' Speech in Early Word Learning , 2010 .

[72]  L. Murray,et al.  Depressed mothers' speech to their infants and its relation to infant gender and cognitive development. , 1993, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[73]  A. Fernald,et al.  A cross-language study of prosodic modifications in mothers' and fathers' speech to preverbal infants , 1989, Journal of Child Language.

[74]  D. Stern,et al.  The prosody of maternal speech: infant age and context related changes , 1983, Journal of Child Language.

[75]  D. Burnham,et al.  Universality and specificity in infant-directed speech: Pitch modifications as a function of infant age and sex in a tonal and non-tonal language , 2001 .

[76]  L. Singh,et al.  Influences of Infant-Directed Speech on Early Word Recognition. , 2009, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[77]  Christine Kitamura,et al.  Age-Specific Preferences for Infant-Directed Affective Intent. , 2009, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[78]  Jeffrey F. Cohn,et al.  Quantitative description and differentiation of fundamental frequency contours , 1994, Comput. Speech Lang..

[79]  A. Fernald,et al.  Prosody and focus in speech to infants and adults , 1991 .

[80]  H. Papoušek,et al.  Didactic adjustments in fathers' and mothers' speech to their 3-month-old infants , 1987 .

[81]  C. Fisher,et al.  The Given-New Contract in Speech to Infants , 1995 .

[82]  P. Kuhl,et al.  Cross-language analysis of phonetic units in language addressed to infants. , 1997, Science.

[83]  Jane L. Abraham,et al.  The development of infants' preference for motherese , 1997 .

[84]  J. Bachorowski,et al.  Infants of Depressed Mothers, Although Competent Learners, Fail to Learn in Response to Their Own Mothers' Infant-Directed Speech , 2002, Psychological science.

[85]  Renate Zangl,et al.  Increased brain activity to infant-directed speech in 6-and 13-month-old infants. , 2007 .

[86]  G. Bryant,et al.  Prosody in Infant-Directed Speech Is Similar Across Western and Traditional Cultures , 2015 .

[87]  Femmie Juffer,et al.  Less is more: meta-analyses of sensitivity and attachment interventions in early childhood. , 2003, Psychological bulletin.

[88]  D. Burnham,et al.  Slow Speech Enhances Younger But Not Older Infants' Perception of Vocal Emotion , 2006 .

[89]  P. Laukka,et al.  Impact of intended emotion intensity on cue utilization and decoding accuracy in vocal expression of emotion. , 2000, Emotion.

[90]  Katsuko Niwano,et al.  Acoustic Determinants Eliciting Japanese Infants' Vocal Response to Maternal Speech , 2002, Psychological reports.

[91]  K. Cheng,et al.  Auditory observation of infant-directed speech by mothers: experience-dependent interaction between language and emotion in the basal ganglia , 2014, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[92]  G. Siegel,et al.  Imitation of intonation by infants. , 1990, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[93]  M. Saxton What's in a name? Coming to terms with the child's linguistic environment , 2008, Journal of Child Language.

[94]  A. Heilbrun,et al.  Maternal Child-Rearing Experience and Self- Reinforcement Effectiveness. , 1970 .

[95]  Katherine Demuth,et al.  Effects of the acoustic properties of infant-directed speech on infant word recognition. , 2010, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[96]  M. Papoušek Early ontogeny of vocal communication in parent-infant interactions , 1992 .

[97]  Erik D. Thiessen,et al.  Infant-Directed Speech Facilitates Word Segmentation. , 2005, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[98]  Leher Singh,et al.  Infants' Listening Preferences: Baby Talk or Happy Talk? , 2002, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[99]  Pitch Characteristics of Speech During Mother-Infant and Father-Infant Vocal Interactions , 2003 .

[100]  James L. Morgan,et al.  Prosody and the Roots of Parsing , 1996 .

[101]  A. Fernald,et al.  Expanded Intonation Contours in Mothers' Speech to Newborns. , 1984 .

[102]  Ruby Sara Coachman Phillips Infant-directed speech in African-American mothers , 1994 .

[103]  Patricia K Kuhl,et al.  Look who's talking: speech style and social context in language input to infants are linked to concurrent and future speech development. , 2014, Developmental science.

[104]  C. Best,et al.  Accommodation in mean f0 during mother–infant and father–infant vocal interactions: a longitudinal case study , 1997, Journal of Child Language.