Does prosody make the difference? A meta-analysis on relations between prosodic aspects of infant-directed speech and infant outcomes.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Herbert L. Pick. Comparative and Developmental Approaches to Spatial Cognition , 1983 .
[2] M. Chetouani,et al. Motherese in Interaction: At the Cross-Road of Emotion and Cognition? (A Systematic Review) , 2013, PloS one.
[3] S Kiritani,et al. Developmental change in auditory preferences for speech stimuli in Japanese infants. , 2001, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.
[4] E. Hoff. The specificity of environmental influence: socioeconomic status affects early vocabulary development via maternal speech. , 2003, Child development.
[5] C. Kitamura,et al. The shift in infant preferences for vowel duration and pitch contour between 6 and 10 months of age. , 2009, Developmental science.
[6] Patricia K. Kuhl,et al. Fast Track , 2020, Electric and Hybrid Vehicle Technology International.
[7] J. Shepherd,et al. The melody of surprise: maternal surprise vocalizations during play with her infant , 2002 .
[8] L. Hedges,et al. Introduction to Meta‐Analysis , 2009, International Coaching Psychology Review.
[9] A. Fernald. Approval and disapproval: infant responsiveness to vocal affect in familiar and unfamiliar languages. , 1993, Child development.
[10] L. D’Odorico,et al. Prosodic and lexical aspects of maternal linguistic input to late-talking toddlers. , 2006, International journal of language & communication disorders.
[11] P. Kaplan,et al. Infant-directed speech produced by fathers with symptoms of depression: effects on infant associative learning in a conditioned-attention paradigm. , 2007, Infant behavior & development.
[12] Maria Uther,et al. Do you speak E-NG-L-I-SH? A comparison of foreigner- and infant-directed speech , 2007, Speech Commun..
[13] Elena E. Lyakso,et al. Infant Vocalizations at the First Year of Life Predict Speech Development at 2 - 7 Years: Longitudinal Study , 2014 .
[14] Adena Schachner,et al. Infant-directed speech drives social preferences in 5-month-old infants. , 2011, Developmental psychology.
[15] Shozo Kojima,et al. Cerebral responses to infant-directed speech and the effect of talker familiarity , 2012, NeuroImage.
[16] A. Fernald,et al. Intonation and communicative intent in mothers' speech to infants: is the melody the message? , 1989, Child development.
[17] S Duval,et al. Trim and Fill: A Simple Funnel‐Plot–Based Method of Testing and Adjusting for Publication Bias in Meta‐Analysis , 2000, Biometrics.
[18] J. Cohn,et al. A combination of vocal fo dynamic and summary features discriminates between three pragmatic categories of infant-directed speech. , 1996, Child development.
[19] H. Gleitman,et al. Mother, Id rather do it myself: Some effects and non-effects of maternal speech style , 1977 .
[20] A. Arteche,et al. Sadness in mothers' 'baby-talk' predicts affective disorder in adolescent offspring. , 2010, Infant behavior & development.
[21] Rochelle S. Newman,et al. Changes in Preference for Infant-Directed Speech in Low and Moderate Noise by 4.5- to 13-Month-Olds. , 2006, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.
[22] Robert Rosenthal,et al. WRITING META-ANALYTIC REVIEWS , 1995 .
[23] Katsuko Niwano,et al. Intonation Contour of Japanese Maternal Infant-Directed Speech and Infant Vocal Response , 2002 .
[24] D. Burnham,et al. What's New, Pussycat? On Talking to Babies and Animals , 2002, Science.
[25] J. Bachorowski,et al. Role of Clinical Diagnosis and Medication Use in Effects of Maternal Depression on Infant-Directed Speech. , 2001, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.
[26] N. Berthier,et al. Preference for infant-directed speech in preterm infants. , 2014, Infant behavior & development.
[27] H. Papoušek,et al. The meanings of melodies in motherese in tone and stress languages , 1991 .
[28] N. Masataka,et al. Pitch characteristics of Japanese maternal speech to infants , 1992, Journal of Child Language.
[29] P. Kaplan,et al. Maternal Sensitivity and the Learning-Promoting Effects of Depressed and Non-Depressed Mothers' Infant-Directed Speech. , 2009, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.
[30] A. Tagini,et al. Linguistic and prosodic aspects of child-directed speech: The role of maternal child-rearing experiences , 2016 .
[31] B. Tabachnick,et al. Using Multivariate Statistics , 1983 .
[32] Catharine H. Echols,et al. Stressed and word-final syllables in infant-directed speech , 1996 .
[33] H. Papoušek,et al. Forms and functions of vocal matching in interactions between mothers and their precanonical infants , 1989 .
[34] Laura L Namy,et al. Say It Like You Mean It: Mothers’ Use of Prosody to Convey Word Meaning , 2012, Language and speech.
[35] M. Goldstein,et al. Habituation, sensitization, and infants' responses to motherese speech. , 1995, Developmental psychobiology.
[36] N. Masataka,et al. Early ontogeny of vocal behavior of Japanese infants in response to maternal speech. , 1992, Child development.
[37] R. Aslin,et al. Developmental differences in infant attention to the spectral properties of infant-directed speech. , 1994, Child development.
[38] D. Stern,et al. Intonation contours as signals in maternal speech to prelinguistic infants. , 1982 .
[39] Klaus R. Scherer,et al. Vocal communication of emotion: A review of research paradigms , 2003, Speech Commun..
[40] C. A. Ferguson,et al. Talking to Children , 1977 .
[41] D. Moore,et al. Categorization of Infant-Directed Speech , 2002 .
[42] A. Fernald. Four-Month-Old Infants Prefer to Listen to Motherese" , 1985 .
[43] D. Burnham,et al. Pitch and Communicative Intent in Mother's Speech: Adjustments for Age and Sex in the First Year , 2003 .
[44] Mohamed Chetouani,et al. Do Parentese Prosody and Fathers' Involvement in Interacting Facilitate Social Interaction in Infants Who Later Develop Autism? , 2013, PloS one.
[45] L. Trainor,et al. Frontal brain electrical activity (EEG) and heart rate in response to affective infant-directed (ID) speech in 9-month-old infants , 2007, Brain and Cognition.
[46] James L. Morgan,et al. Is early word-form processing stress-full? How natural variability supports recognition , 2010, Cognitive Psychology.
[47] Alejandrina Cristià,et al. Input to Language: The Phonetics and Perception of Infant-Directed Speech , 2013, Lang. Linguistics Compass.
[48] L. Trainor,et al. Is Infant-Directed Speech Prosody a Result of the Vocal Expression of Emotion? , 2000, Psychological science.
[49] Kathy Hirsh-Pasek,et al. Word Learning in Infant- and Adult-Directed Speech , 2011, Language learning and development : the official journal of the Society for Language Development.
[50] C. A. Ferguson,et al. Talking to Children: Language Input and Acquisition , 1979 .
[51] M. Gratier,et al. Imitation and repetition of prosodic contour in vocal interaction at 3 months. , 2011, Developmental psychology.
[52] J. Culling,et al. Parental Speech at 6 Months Predicts Joint Attention at 12 Months , 2013 .
[53] J. Bachorowski,et al. Child-directed speech produced by mothers with symptoms of depression fails to promote associative learning in 4-month-old infants. , 1999, Child development.
[54] T. Walker,et al. The Effects Of Parental Behaviors And Prosody On The Language And Cognitive Development Of Infants , 2012 .
[55] M. Soderstrom,et al. Entrainment of prosody in the interaction of mothers with their young children* , 2015, Journal of Child Language.
[56] M. Rowe,et al. Child-directed speech: relation to socioeconomic status, knowledge of child development and child vocabulary skill* , 2008, Journal of Child Language.
[57] J. Morgan,et al. Acoustical cues and grammatical units in speech to two preverbal infants* , 2008, Journal of Child Language.
[58] Kathy Hirsh-Pasek,et al. (Baby)Talk to Me , 2015 .
[59] L. Murray,et al. The effects of maternal depression on the 'musicality' of infant-directed speech and conversational engagement , 2010 .
[60] Kuniaki Sugai,et al. Maternal Accommodation in Infant-Directed Speech during Mother's and Twin-Infants' Vocal Interactions , 2003, Psychological reports.
[61] C. A. Ferguson. Baby Talk in Six Languages , 1964 .
[62] M. Soderstrom,et al. Beyond babytalk: Re-evaluating the nature and content of speech input to preverbal infants , 2007 .
[63] B. Bettes,et al. Maternal depression and motherese: temporal and intonational features. , 1988, Child development.
[64] P. Kuhl,et al. Acoustic determinants of infant preference for motherese speech , 1987 .
[65] N. Zaretskaya,et al. Introspection, attention or awareness? The role of the frontal lobe in binocular rivalry , 2014, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[66] J. Bachorowski,et al. Depression Diagnoses and Fundamental Frequency-Based Acoustic Cues in Maternal Infant-Directed Speech , 2014, Language learning and development : the official journal of the Society for Language Development.
[67] Mohamed Chetouani,et al. Course of maternal prosodic incitation (motherese) during early development in autism: An exploratory home movie study , 2013 .
[68] Monja A. Knoll,et al. Characterising F(0) contour shape in infant- and foreigner-directed speech , 2015, Speech Commun..
[69] P. Kuhl,et al. Maternal speech to infants in a tonal language: Support for universal prosodic features in motherese. , 1988 .
[70] G. Csibra,et al. Gaze Following in Human Infants Depends on Communicative Signals , 2008, Current Biology.
[71] Michael C. Frank,et al. Contributions of Prosodic and Distributional Features of Caregivers' Speech in Early Word Learning , 2010 .
[72] L. Murray,et al. Depressed mothers' speech to their infants and its relation to infant gender and cognitive development. , 1993, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.
[73] A. Fernald,et al. A cross-language study of prosodic modifications in mothers' and fathers' speech to preverbal infants , 1989, Journal of Child Language.
[74] D. Stern,et al. The prosody of maternal speech: infant age and context related changes , 1983, Journal of Child Language.
[75] D. Burnham,et al. Universality and specificity in infant-directed speech: Pitch modifications as a function of infant age and sex in a tonal and non-tonal language , 2001 .
[76] L. Singh,et al. Influences of Infant-Directed Speech on Early Word Recognition. , 2009, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.
[77] Christine Kitamura,et al. Age-Specific Preferences for Infant-Directed Affective Intent. , 2009, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.
[78] Jeffrey F. Cohn,et al. Quantitative description and differentiation of fundamental frequency contours , 1994, Comput. Speech Lang..
[79] A. Fernald,et al. Prosody and focus in speech to infants and adults , 1991 .
[80] H. Papoušek,et al. Didactic adjustments in fathers' and mothers' speech to their 3-month-old infants , 1987 .
[81] C. Fisher,et al. The Given-New Contract in Speech to Infants , 1995 .
[82] P. Kuhl,et al. Cross-language analysis of phonetic units in language addressed to infants. , 1997, Science.
[83] Jane L. Abraham,et al. The development of infants' preference for motherese , 1997 .
[84] J. Bachorowski,et al. Infants of Depressed Mothers, Although Competent Learners, Fail to Learn in Response to Their Own Mothers' Infant-Directed Speech , 2002, Psychological science.
[85] Renate Zangl,et al. Increased brain activity to infant-directed speech in 6-and 13-month-old infants. , 2007 .
[86] G. Bryant,et al. Prosody in Infant-Directed Speech Is Similar Across Western and Traditional Cultures , 2015 .
[87] Femmie Juffer,et al. Less is more: meta-analyses of sensitivity and attachment interventions in early childhood. , 2003, Psychological bulletin.
[88] D. Burnham,et al. Slow Speech Enhances Younger But Not Older Infants' Perception of Vocal Emotion , 2006 .
[89] P. Laukka,et al. Impact of intended emotion intensity on cue utilization and decoding accuracy in vocal expression of emotion. , 2000, Emotion.
[90] Katsuko Niwano,et al. Acoustic Determinants Eliciting Japanese Infants' Vocal Response to Maternal Speech , 2002, Psychological reports.
[91] K. Cheng,et al. Auditory observation of infant-directed speech by mothers: experience-dependent interaction between language and emotion in the basal ganglia , 2014, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[92] G. Siegel,et al. Imitation of intonation by infants. , 1990, Journal of speech and hearing research.
[93] M. Saxton. What's in a name? Coming to terms with the child's linguistic environment , 2008, Journal of Child Language.
[94] A. Heilbrun,et al. Maternal Child-Rearing Experience and Self- Reinforcement Effectiveness. , 1970 .
[95] Katherine Demuth,et al. Effects of the acoustic properties of infant-directed speech on infant word recognition. , 2010, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[96] M. Papoušek. Early ontogeny of vocal communication in parent-infant interactions , 1992 .
[97] Erik D. Thiessen,et al. Infant-Directed Speech Facilitates Word Segmentation. , 2005, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.
[98] Leher Singh,et al. Infants' Listening Preferences: Baby Talk or Happy Talk? , 2002, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.
[99] Pitch Characteristics of Speech During Mother-Infant and Father-Infant Vocal Interactions , 2003 .
[100] James L. Morgan,et al. Prosody and the Roots of Parsing , 1996 .
[101] A. Fernald,et al. Expanded Intonation Contours in Mothers' Speech to Newborns. , 1984 .
[102] Ruby Sara Coachman Phillips. Infant-directed speech in African-American mothers , 1994 .
[103] Patricia K Kuhl,et al. Look who's talking: speech style and social context in language input to infants are linked to concurrent and future speech development. , 2014, Developmental science.
[104] C. Best,et al. Accommodation in mean f0 during mother–infant and father–infant vocal interactions: a longitudinal case study , 1997, Journal of Child Language.