Making your next move: Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and planning a sequence of actions in freely moving monkeys

Prefrontal damage disrupts planning, as measured by disorders of the activities of daily living (Humphreys & Forde, 1998; Shallice & Burgess, 1991). In a monkey model of this form of planning, a variant of the delayed alternation task was performed by freely moving monkeys. In a 16×16-ft. testing room, four feeders were located in the middle of each wall. In the north task, monkeys alternated between feeders: west-north-east-north-west, and so forth. In the south task, the alternation sequence was east-south-west-south-east, and so forth. Neuronal activity was recorded during walking along the eight paths, constituting the north and south tasks. To succeed, monkeys had to memorize the alternation rule and monitor both their place in the sequence and the previously made spatially directed action before deciding to walk to a new location to the left or right of the current location. Responsive dorsolateral prefrontal neurons are strikingly selective. Sustained neuronal activity reflects the spatial direction of an ongoing or upcoming response. It is important that such selective responses occur in one but not both tasks, even though the movements are exactly the same in both tasks and at each location. We suggest that selective neuronal activity is tuned through learning and reflects the fundamental units of a planning mechanism: Individual neurons encode specific components of a sequence of behavioral actions and their temporal order. Populations of such neurons represent all the steps necessary to perform the north and south tasks. The sustained activity of these neurons suggests that planning and working memory mechanisms are integrated.

[1]  S. Funahashi,et al.  Delay-period activity in the primate prefrontal cortex encoding multiple spatial positions and their order of presentation , 1997, Behavioural Brain Research.

[2]  C. Gross,et al.  Evidence for dissociation of impairment on auditory discrimination and delayed response following lateral frontal lesions in monkeys. , 1962, Experimental neurology.

[3]  M MISHKIN,et al.  Effects of small frontal lesions on delayed alternation in monkeys. , 1957, Journal of neurophysiology.

[4]  J. Fuster,et al.  Cellular discharge in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex of the monkey in cognitive tasks , 1982, Experimental Neurology.

[5]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Segregation of working memory functions within the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[6]  J. Fuster The Prefrontal Cortex , 1997 .

[7]  P S Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Cytoarchitectonic definition of prefrontal areas in the normal human cortex: I. Remapping of areas 9 and 46 using quantitative criteria. , 1995, Cerebral cortex.

[8]  T. Shallice,et al.  Deficits in strategy application following frontal lobe damage in man. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[9]  N Butters,et al.  Retention of Delayed-Alternation: Effect of Selective Lesions of Sulcus Principalis , 1969, Science.

[10]  B. Milner,et al.  Deficits on subject-ordered tasks after frontal- and temporal-lobe lesions in man , 1982, Neuropsychologia.

[11]  F. Richer,et al.  Inter-response interference contributes to the sequencing deficit in frontal lobe lesions. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[12]  D. Stuss,et al.  The Frontal Lobes , 1986 .

[13]  J. Taube Head direction cells and the neurophysiological basis for a sense of direction , 1998, Progress in Neurobiology.

[14]  M. W. Montgomery,et al.  The Quantitative Description of Action Disorganisation after Brain Damage: A Case Study , 1991 .

[15]  J. Joseph,et al.  Prefrontal cortex and spatial sequencing in macaque monkey , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[16]  C C Hilgetag,et al.  Quantitative architecture distinguishes prefrontal cortical systems in the rhesus monkey. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.

[17]  H Niki,et al.  Prefrontal unit activity during delayed alternation in the monkey. I. Relation to direction of response. , 1974, Brain research.

[18]  Robert T. Knight,et al.  Effects of frontal lobe damage on interference effects in working memory , 2002, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[19]  D. Stuss,et al.  Capture errors and sequencing after frontal brain lesions , 1993, Neuropsychologia.

[20]  J. Tanji,et al.  Representation of the temporal order of visual objects in the primate lateral prefrontal cortex. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[21]  A. Mikami,et al.  Neuronal responses to photographs in the superior temporal sulcus of the rhesus monkey , 1994, Behavioural Brain Research.

[22]  R. Malmo INTERFERENCE FACTORS IN DELAYED RESPONSE IN MONKEYS AFTER REMOVAL OF FRONTAL LOBES , 1942 .

[23]  G. Humphreys,et al.  Disordered action schema and action disorganisation syndrome , 1998 .

[24]  Anne B. Sereno,et al.  Antisaccades and smooth pursuit eye movements in schizophrenia , 1995, Biological Psychiatry.

[25]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Coactivation of prefrontal cortex and inferior parietal cortex in working memory tasks revealed by 2DG functional mapping in the rhesus monkey , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[26]  Yuanye Ma,et al.  Telemetric recordings of single neuron activity and visual scenes in monkeys walking in an open field , 2004, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[27]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Posterior parietal cortex in rhesus monkey: I. Parcellation of areas based on distinctive limbic and sensory corticocortical connections , 1989, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[28]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Transient and sustained activity in a distributed neural system for human working memory , 1997, Nature.

[29]  Bradley R Postle,et al.  The dependence of span and delayed-response performance on prefrontal cortex , 1999, Neuropsychologia.

[30]  F. A. Wilson,et al.  Functional stability of dorsolateral prefrontal neurons. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[31]  H. Tanila,et al.  Dissociation of mnemonic coding and other functional neuronal processing in the monkey prefrontal cortex. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[32]  Glyn W Humphreys,et al.  AGNOSIA WITHOUT PROSOPAGNOSIA OR ALEXIA: EVIDENCE FOR STORED VISUAL MEMORIES SPECIFIC TO OBJECTS. , 1998, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[33]  H. E. Rosvold,et al.  Localization of function within the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex of the rhesus monkey. , 1970, Experimental neurology.

[34]  A. Sirigu,et al.  Distinct Frontal Regions for Processing Sentence Syntax and Story Grammar , 1998, Cortex.

[35]  F. A. Wilson,et al.  A microelectrode drive for long term recording of neurons in freely moving and chaired monkeys , 2003, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[36]  A. Baddeley Exploring the Central Executive , 1996 .

[37]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Mnemonic coding of visual space in the monkey's dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.

[38]  C. Jacobsen FUNCTIONS OF FRONTAL ASSOCIATION AREA IN PRIMATES , 1935 .

[39]  J. Jonides,et al.  Storage and executive processes in the frontal lobes. , 1999, Science.

[40]  Yuan-Ye Ma,et al.  Spatially directed movement and neuronal activity in freely moving monkey. , 2004, Progress in brain research.

[41]  J. O’Keefe Do hippocampal pyramidal cells signal non‐spatial as well as spatial information? , 1999, Hippocampus.

[42]  T. Shallice,et al.  Action Sequencing Deficit Following Frontal Lobe Lesion , 2002, Neurocase.

[43]  Yuan-Ye Ma,et al.  An automated food delivery system for behavioral and neurophysiological studies of learning and memory in freely moving monkeys , 2005, Behavior research methods.

[44]  P S Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Face-selective neurons during passive viewing and working memory performance of rhesus monkeys: evidence for intrinsic specialization of neuronal coding. , 1999, Cerebral cortex.