Single- and cross-commodity discounting among cocaine addicts: the commodity and its temporal location determine discounting rate
暂无分享,去创建一个
Zeb Kurth-Nelson | A. David Redish | Warren K. Bickel | Reid D. Landes | Z. Kurth-Nelson | A. Redish | W. Bickel | R. Landes | Bryan A. Jones | D. Christensen | Lisa Jackson | D. Christensen | Lisa Jackson | Darren R. Christensen | L. Jackson | W. K. Bickel | R. D. Landes | B. A. Jones | Z. Kurth-Nelson | A David Redish | A D Redish
[1] Paul F. Hill,et al. Remember the Future: Working Memory Training Decreases Delay Discounting Among Stimulant Addicts , 2011, Biological Psychiatry.
[2] W. Bickel,et al. Hypothetical intertemporal choice and real economic behavior: delay discounting predicts voucher redemptions during contingency-management procedures. , 2010, Experimental and clinical psychopharmacology.
[3] P. Glimcher. Foundations of Neuroeconomic Analysis , 2010 .
[4] W. Bickel,et al. Analytical methods to detect within-individual changes in discounting. , 2010, Experimental and clinical psychopharmacology.
[5] P. Glimcher,et al. The Neurobiology of Decision: Consensus and Controversy , 2009, Neuron.
[6] W. Bickel,et al. Buprenorphine medication versus voucher contingencies in promoting abstinence from opioids and cocaine. , 2009, Experimental and clinical psychopharmacology.
[7] Warren K Bickel,et al. Congruence of BOLD Response across Intertemporal Choice Conditions: Fictive and Real Money Gains and Losses , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[8] E. T. Mueller,et al. Toward the Study of Trans-Disease Processes: A Novel Approach With Special Reference to the Study of Co-morbidity , 2009, Journal of dual diagnosis.
[9] G. Badger,et al. Changes in the relative reinforcing effects of cigarette smoking as a function of initial abstinence , 2009, Psychopharmacology.
[10] P. Glimcher. Understanding risk: A guide for the perplexed , 2008, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.
[11] R. Weller,et al. Obese women show greater delay discounting than healthy-weight women , 2008, Appetite.
[12] A. Odum,et al. Corrigendum to “Cigarette smokers show steeper discounting of both food and cigarettes than money” [Drug Alcohol Depend. 91 (2/3) (2007) 293–296] , 2008 .
[13] Shawn R. Charlton,et al. Commodity specific rates of temporal discounting: Does metabolic function underlie differences in rates of discounting? , 2008, Behavioural Processes.
[14] W. John Boscardin,et al. SAS® for mixed models (2nd edn). Ramon C. Littell, George A. Milliken, Walter W. Stroup, Russell D. Wolfinger and Oliver Schabenberger, SAS Institute, Cary, NC, 2006. No. of pages: xii + 814. Price: $89.95. ISBN 10: 1-59047-500-3; ISBN 13: 978-1-59047-500-3 , 2007 .
[15] A. Odum,et al. Cigarette smokers show steeper discounting of both food and cigarettes than money. , 2007, Drug and alcohol dependence.
[16] W. Bickel,et al. Behavioral and neuroeconomics of drug addiction: competing neural systems and temporal discounting processes. , 2007, Drug and alcohol dependence.
[17] Daniel D. Holt,et al. Discounting of Monetary and Directly Consumable Rewards , 2007, Psychological science.
[18] S. Mitchell,et al. Neuropsychological function and delay discounting in methamphetamine-dependent individuals , 2006, Psychopharmacology.
[19] Matthew W. Johnson,et al. Delay discounting in currently using and currently abstinent cocaine-dependent outpatients and non-drug-using matched controls. , 2006, Addictive behaviors.
[20] S. Mitchell,et al. Delay discounting predicts behavioral sensitization to ethanol in outbred WSC mice. , 2006, Alcoholism, clinical and experimental research.
[21] S. Mitchell,et al. Effects of short-term nicotine deprivation on decision-making: delay, uncertainty and effort discounting. , 2004, Nicotine & tobacco research : official journal of the Society for Research on Nicotine and Tobacco.
[22] D. Hommer,et al. Impulsivity in abstinent alcohol-dependent patients: relation to control subjects and type 1-/type 2-like traits. , 2004, Alcohol.
[23] Kris N Kirby,et al. Heroin and cocaine abusers have higher discount rates for delayed rewards than alcoholics or non-drug-using controls. , 2004, Addiction.
[24] Matthew W. Johnson,et al. Delay discounting in current and never-before cigarette smokers: similarities and differences across commodity, sign, and magnitude. , 2003, Journal of abnormal psychology.
[25] K. Brady,et al. Impulsivity and rapid discounting of delayed hypothetical rewards in cocaine-dependent individuals. , 2003, Experimental and clinical psychopharmacology.
[26] Joel Myerson,et al. Cross-Cultural Comparisons of Discounting Delayed and Probabilistic Rewards , 2002 .
[27] Amy L Odum,et al. Discounting of delayed health gains and losses by current, never- and ex-smokers of cigarettes. , 2002, Nicotine & tobacco research : official journal of the Society for Research on Nicotine and Tobacco.
[28] Warren K Bickel,et al. Within-subject comparison of real and hypothetical money rewards in delay discounting. , 2002, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[29] N. Petry. Pathological gamblers, with and without substance use disorders, discount delayed rewards at high rates. , 2001, Journal of abnormal psychology.
[30] A. Odum,et al. Impulsivity and cigarette smoking: delay discounting in current, never, and ex-smokers , 1999, Psychopharmacology.
[31] S. Mitchell,et al. Measures of impulsivity in cigarette smokers and non-smokers , 1999, Psychopharmacology.
[32] G. Madden,et al. Discounting of delayed rewards in opioid-dependent outpatients: exponential or hyperbolic discounting functions? , 1999, Experimental and clinical psychopharmacology.
[33] W. Bickel,et al. Heroin addicts have higher discount rates for delayed rewards than non-drug-using controls. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[34] G. Badger,et al. Effects of adding behavioral treatment to opioid detoxification with buprenorphine. , 1997, Journal of consulting and clinical psychology.
[35] W K Bickel,et al. Impulsive and self-control choices in opioid-dependent patients and non-drug-using control participants: drug and monetary rewards. , 1997, Experimental and clinical psychopharmacology.
[36] K. Kirby,et al. Bidding on the Future: Evidence Against Normative Discounting of Delayed Rewards , 1997 .
[37] Joel Myerson,et al. Exponential Versus Hyperbolic Discounting of Delayed Outcomes: Risk and Waiting Time , 1996 .
[38] G. Badger,et al. Incentives improve outcome in outpatient behavioral treatment of cocaine dependence. , 1994, Archives of general psychiatry.
[39] S. Tiffany,et al. A cognitive model of drug urges and drug-use behavior: role of automatic and nonautomatic processes. , 1990, Psychological review.
[40] A. Logue. Research on self-control: An integrating framework , 1988, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[41] A. Tversky,et al. The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. , 1981, Science.
[42] W. Hulstijn,et al. Impulsivity in abstinent early- and late-onset alcoholics: differences in self-report measures and a discounting task. , 2006, Addiction.
[43] W. Bickel,et al. Toward a behavioral economic understanding of drug dependence: delay discounting processes. , 2001, Addiction.
[44] F. Rotgers,et al. Treating substance abuse : theory and technique , 1996 .
[45] A. Tversky,et al. Prospect theory: analysis of decision under risk , 1979 .
[46] B. Pitt. Psychopharmacology , 1968, Mental Health.
[47] D. Bernoulli. Exposition of a New Theory on the Measurement of Risk , 1954 .