Extracting the orientation of rotating objects without object identification: Object orientation induction.

When a rotating object (inducer) is briefly replaced by a static face image (test stimulus), the orientation of the face appears to shift in the rotation direction of the inducer (object orientation induction, OOI). The OOI effect suggests that there is a process to continuously analyze and update the orientation of an object in motion. We investigated the perception of object orientation in motion, examining potential factors that contribute to OOI. Experiment 1 showed that the phenomenon is general to objects rather than specific to faces; OOI could be observed with non-face objects. Experiment 2 showed that OOI is a 3D effect, as the orientation shift for a bent-wire object depended on its configuration in the depth dimension. Experiment 3 showed that salient features are necessary to indicate the intrinsic orientation of the inducing object for producing OOI. Experiment 4 showed that change in the facing direction of the inducer object is a crucial factor for OOI, but neither the object shape nor its identity is important. A strong OOI effect was observed even when the inducer kept changing its shape and identity, as long as its direction change generated continuous rotation. Finally, Experiment 5 showed that OOI is a phenomenon in the pathway for fast visual processing. A single inducer presented shorter than 100ms before influenced the perceived orientation of the test stimulus. Together these results suggest that there is a predictive process that continuously analyzes and updates the orientation of rotating objects, independently of their identification.

[1]  I. Biederman Recognizing depth-rotated objects: a review of recent research and theory. , 2000, Spatial vision.

[2]  Kazumichi Matsumiya,et al.  Implicit Learning of Viewpoint-Independent Spatial Layouts , 2012, Front. Psychology.

[3]  Michael J. Berry,et al.  Anticipation of moving stimuli by the retina , 1999, Nature.

[4]  S. Edelman,et al.  Differential Processing of Objects under Various Viewing Conditions in the Human Lateral Occipital Complex , 1999, Neuron.

[5]  D H Kelly,et al.  Motion and vision. I. Stabilized images of stationary gratings. , 1979, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[6]  Felix Wichmann,et al.  The psychometric function: II. Bootstrap-based confidence intervals and sampling , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.

[7]  E. Rolls,et al.  View-invariant representations of familiar objects by neurons in the inferior temporal visual cortex. , 1998, Cerebral cortex.

[8]  Hirohisa Yaguchi,et al.  Measuring attention using flash-lag effect. , 2010, Journal of vision.

[9]  T J Sejnowski,et al.  Motion integration and postdiction in visual awareness. , 2000, Science.

[10]  Masayuki Kobayashi,et al.  Spatial representations of the viewer’s surroundings , 2018, Scientific Reports.

[11]  Michael J. Berry,et al.  Predictive information in a sensory population , 2013, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[12]  M. Chun,et al.  Contextual Cueing: Implicit Learning and Memory of Visual Context Guides Spatial Attention , 1998, Cognitive Psychology.

[13]  Kazumichi Matsumiya,et al.  Looking off effect – shift of face direction caused by a rotating object , 2010 .

[14]  I. Murakami,et al.  Latency difference, not spatial extrapolation , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.

[15]  I. Biederman,et al.  Recognizing depth-rotated objects: evidence and conditions for three-dimensional viewpoint invariance. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[16]  P. Thompson,et al.  Margaret Thatcher: A New Illusion , 1980, Perception.

[17]  I. Murakami,et al.  A flash-lag effect in random motion , 2001, Vision Research.

[18]  S. Edelman,et al.  Orientation dependence in the recognition of familiar and novel views of three-dimensional objects , 1992, Vision Research.

[19]  Dave Bartram,et al.  The role of visual and semantic codes in object naming , 1974 .

[20]  I. Biederman Recognition-by-components: a theory of human image understanding. , 1987, Psychological review.

[21]  D. H. Kelly Motion and vision. II. Stabilized spatio-temporal threshold surface. , 1979, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[22]  Patrick Cavanagh,et al.  The flash grab effect , 2012, Vision Research.

[23]  K. D. De Valois,et al.  Vernier acuity with stationary moving Gabors. , 1991, Vision research.

[24]  T. Poggio,et al.  A network that learns to recognize three-dimensional objects , 1990, Nature.

[25]  Moshe Bar,et al.  Viewpoint Dependency in Visual Object Recognition Does Not Necessarily Imply Viewer-Centered Representation , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[26]  Romi Nijhawan,et al.  Motion extrapolation in catching , 1994, Nature.

[27]  H H Bülthoff,et al.  Psychophysical support for a two-dimensional view interpolation theory of object recognition. , 1992, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[28]  H DE LANGE DZN,et al.  Research into the dynamic nature of the human fovea-cortex systems with intermittent and modulated light. II. Phase shift in brithtness and delay in color perception. , 1958, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[29]  V. Ramachandran,et al.  Illusory Displacement of Equiluminous Kinetic Edges , 1990, Perception.

[30]  H DE LANGE DZN,et al.  Research into the dynamic nature of the human fovea-cortex systems with intermittent and modulated light. I. Attenuation characteristics with white and colored light. , 1958, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[31]  Sheng He,et al.  Viewer-Centered Object Representation in the Human Visual System Revealed by Viewpoint Aftereffects , 2005, Neuron.

[32]  P. Cavanagh,et al.  ISI produces reverse apparent motion , 1990, Vision Research.

[33]  N. Logothetis,et al.  View-dependent object recognition by monkeys , 1994, Current Biology.

[34]  H. D. L. Dzn Research into the dynamic nature of the human fovea-cortex systems with intermittent and modulated light. I. Attenuation characteristics with white and colored light. , 1958 .

[35]  David Whitney,et al.  Motion distorts visual space: shifting the perceived position of remote stationary objects , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[36]  R. Weale Vision. A Computational Investigation Into the Human Representation and Processing of Visual Information. David Marr , 1983 .