Competing Explanations of Competing Explanations: Accounting for Conflict Between Scientific and Folk Explanations

People who hold scientific explanations for natural phenomena also hold folk explanations, and the two types of explanations compete under some circumstances. Here, we explore the question of why folk explanations persist in the face of a well-understood scientific alternative, a phenomenon known as explanatory coexistence. We consider two accounts: an associative account, where coexistence is driven by low-level associations between co-occurring ideas in experience or discourse, and a theory-based account, where coexistence reflects high-level competition between distinct sets of causal expectations. We present data that assess the relative contributions of these two accounts to the cognitive conflict elicited by counterintuitive scientific ideas. Participants (134 college undergraduates) verified scientific statements like "air has weight" and "bacteria have DNA" as quickly as possible, and we examined the speed and accuracy of their verifications in relation to measures of associative information (lexical co-occurrence of the statements' subjects and predicates) and theory-based expectations (ratings of whether the statements' subjects possess theory-relevant attributes). Both measures explained a significant amount of variance in participants' responses, but the theory-based measures explained three to five times more. These data suggest that the cognitive conflict elicited by counterintuitive scientific ideas typically arises from competing theories and that such ideas might be made more intuitive by strengthening scientific theories or weakening folk theories.

[1]  G. Hatano,et al.  Young children's naive theory of biology , 1994, Cognition.

[2]  Martin Riopel,et al.  Is inhibition involved in overcoming a common physics misconception in mechanics? , 2015, Trends in Neuroscience and Education.

[3]  S. Ohlsson,et al.  Resubsumption: A Possible Mechanism for Conceptual Change and Belief Revision , 2009 .

[4]  Nikos Makris,et al.  The Recruitment of Shifting and Inhibition in On-line Science and Mathematics Tasks , 2018, Cogn. Sci..

[5]  N. Epley,et al.  Science and God: An automatic opposition between ultimate explanations , 2009 .

[6]  Ruth Stavy,et al.  Persistence of the Intuitive Conception of Living Things in Adolescence , 2010 .

[7]  A. Gopnik,et al.  Reconstructing constructivism: causal models, Bayesian learning mechanisms, and the theory theory. , 2012, Psychological bulletin.

[8]  Andrew Shtulman,et al.  Tensions Between Science and Intuition Across the Lifespan , 2016, Top. Cogn. Sci..

[9]  P. Harris,et al.  Children's Acceptance of Conflicting Testimony: The Case of Death , 2005 .

[10]  T. Landauer,et al.  A Solution to Plato's Problem: The Latent Semantic Analysis Theory of Acquisition, Induction, and Representation of Knowledge. , 1997 .

[11]  Susan A. Gelman,et al.  Bewitchment, Biology, or Both: The Co-Existence of Natural and Supernatural Explanatory Frameworks Across Development , 2008, Cogn. Sci..

[12]  A. Feeney,et al.  The engine of thought is a hybrid: roles of associative and structured knowledge in reasoning. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[13]  K. Rosengren,et al.  Causal Reasoning about Illness: A Comparison between European- and Vietnamese-American Children , 2004 .

[14]  P. Harris,et al.  Does the Body Survive Death? Cultural Variation in Beliefs About Life Everlasting. , 2017, Cognitive science.

[15]  Andrew Shtulman,et al.  Scientific knowledge suppresses but does not supplant earlier intuitions , 2012, Cognition.

[16]  Tania Lombrozo,et al.  Evidence of a Preference for Teleological Explanations in Patients With Alzheimer’s Disease , 2007 .

[17]  A. Landrum,et al.  No Missing Link: Knowledge Predicts Acceptance of Evolution in the United States , 2018, BioScience.

[18]  C. Legare,et al.  Explanatory pluralism across cultures and development , 2018 .

[19]  M. Chi,et al.  From things to processes: A theory of conceptual change for learning science concepts , 1994 .

[20]  Carol L. Smith Bootstrapping Processes in the Development of Students' Commonsense Matter Theories: Using Analogical Mappings, Thought Experiments, and Learning to Measure to Promote Conceptual Restructuring , 2007 .

[21]  T. Lombrozo,et al.  A Coexistence View of Conceptual Change , 2016 .

[22]  Andrew Shtulman,et al.  Can Science Beat Out Intuition? Increasing the Accessibility of Counterintuitive Scientific Ideas , 2018, CogSci.

[23]  Yilmaz Saglam,et al.  Middle school students' beliefs about matter , 2005 .

[24]  M. Linn,et al.  How do students' views of science influence knowledge integration? , 1991 .

[25]  John J. Clement,et al.  Using Bridging Analogies and Anchoring Institutions to Seal with Students' Preconceptions in Physics , 1993 .

[26]  S. Sloman The empirical case for two systems of reasoning. , 1996 .

[27]  Martin Riopel,et al.  Differences in Brain Activation between Novices and Experts in Science during a Task Involving a Common Misconception in Electricity. , 2014 .

[28]  M. Linn,et al.  Heat energy and temperature concepts of adolescents, adults, and experts: Implications for curricular improvements , 1994 .

[29]  Jesse L. Preston,et al.  Neuroscience and the soul: Competing explanations for the human experience , 2013, Cognition.

[30]  F. Keil Concepts, Kinds, and Cognitive Development , 1989 .

[31]  E. Spelke,et al.  Science and Core Knowledge , 1996, Philosophy of Science.

[32]  S. Thompson-Schill,et al.  Developmental “Roots” in Mature Biological Knowledge , 2009, Psychological science.

[33]  Cristine H. Legare,et al.  Interdisciplinary and Cross-Cultural Perspectives on Explanatory Coexistence , 2015, Top. Cogn. Sci..

[34]  S. Gelman,et al.  A Cross-Cultural Developmental Analysis of Children's and Adults' Understanding of Illness in South Asia (India) and the United States , 2004 .

[35]  Andrew Shtulman,et al.  Children’s Ability to Learn Evolutionary Explanations for Biological Adaptation , 2016, Young Children’s Developing Understanding of the Biological World.

[36]  Bruce L Sherin,et al.  A Computational Study of Commonsense Science: An Exploration in the Automated Analysis of Clinical Interview Data , 2013 .

[37]  Gregg E. A. Solomon,et al.  Animist thinking in the elderly and in patients with Alzheimer's disease , 2008, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[38]  Medha Tare,et al.  A Conceptual Guide to Natural History Museum Visitors’ Understanding of Evolution , 2009 .

[39]  Martin Riopel,et al.  Experimental evidence of the superiority of the prevalence model of conceptual change over the classical models and repetition , 2015 .

[40]  Caitlin F. Canfield,et al.  The divided mind of a disbeliever: Intuitive beliefs about nature as purposefully created among different groups of non-religious adults , 2015, Cognition.

[41]  Joshua Rottman,et al.  Journal of Experimental Psychology : General Professional Physical Scientists Display Tenacious Teleological Tendencies : Purpose-Based Reasoning as a Cognitive Default , 2012 .

[42]  Patrice Potvin,et al.  Toward a durable prevalence of scientific conceptions: Tracking the effects of two interfering misconceptions about buoyancy from preschoolers to science teachers , 2017 .

[43]  C. Legare,et al.  The coexistence of natural and supernatural explanations within and across domains and development , 2017, The British journal of developmental psychology.

[44]  C. Legare,et al.  Between Religion and Science: Integrating Psychological and Philosophical Accounts of Explanatory Coexistence , 2011, Human Development.

[45]  K. Koschutnig,et al.  Individual differences in science competence among students are associated with ventrolateral prefrontal cortex activation , 2019, Journal of neuroscience research.

[46]  Jessica L. Tracy,et al.  Death and Science: The Existential Underpinnings of Belief in Intelligent Design and Discomfort with Evolution , 2011, PloS one.

[47]  Tamsin C. German,et al.  Representational coexistence in the God concept: Core knowledge intuitions of God as a person are not revised by Christian theology despite lifelong experience , 2018, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

[48]  D. Kelemen,et al.  The Human Function Compunction: Teleological explanation in adults , 2009, Cognition.

[49]  Matteo Colombo,et al.  Experimental Philosophy of Explanation Rising: The Case for a Plurality of Concepts of Explanation , 2017, Cogn. Sci..

[50]  D. Medin,et al.  The role of theories in conceptual coherence. , 1985, Psychological review.

[51]  Jonathan Evans Dual-processing accounts of reasoning, judgment, and social cognition. , 2008, Annual review of psychology.

[52]  Denis Mareschal,et al.  The neuroscience of conceptual learning in science and mathematics , 2016, Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences.

[53]  Ruth Stavy,et al.  Language and Children's Conceptions of Plants as Living Things. , 1992 .

[54]  Jonathan D. Lane,et al.  Developing Concepts of the Mind, Body, and Afterlife: Exploring the Roles of Narrative Context and Culture , 2016 .

[55]  Cristine H Legare,et al.  Concepts and folk theories. , 2011, Annual review of anthropology.

[56]  K. Rosengren,et al.  The coexistence of natural and supernatural explanations across cultures and development. , 2012, Child development.

[57]  Stefan Kaufmann,et al.  Computer assessment of interview data using latent semantic analysis , 2008, Behavior research methods.

[58]  S. Vosniadou Capturing and modeling the process of conceptual change. , 1994 .

[59]  Andrew Shtulman,et al.  Developing A Cognitive Reflection Test for School-Age Children , 2018, CogSci.

[60]  H. Wellman,et al.  Cognitive development: foundational theories of core domains. , 1992, Annual review of psychology.

[61]  V. Sloutsky,et al.  Attentional learning and flexible induction: how mundane mechanisms give rise to smart behaviors. , 2008, Child development.