Speech Shadowing While Driving

We investigated the role of cross-modal links in spatial attention in modulating the efficiency of dual-task performance. The difficulty of combining speech shadowing with a simulated driving task was modulated by the spatial location from which the speech was presented. In both single- and dual-task conditions, participants found it significantly easier to shadow one of two auditory streams when the relevant speech was presented from directly in front of them, rather than from the side. This frontal speech advantage was more pronounced when participants performed the demanding simulated driving task at the same time as shadowing than when they performed the shadowing task alone. These results demonstrate that people process auditory information more efficiently (with a lower overall dual-task decrement) when relevant auditory and visual stimuli are presented from the same, rather than different, spatial locations. These results are related to recent findings showing that there are extensive cross-modal links in spatial attention, and have clear implications for the design of better user interfaces.

[1]  C. L. M. The Psychology of Attention , 1890, Nature.

[2]  E. Poulton,et al.  MEASURING THE SPARE ‘MENTAL CAPACITY ’OF CAR DRIVERS BY A SUBSIDIARY TASK , 1961 .

[3]  I. Brown,et al.  Interference between concurrent tasks of driving and telephoning. , 1969, The Journal of applied psychology.

[4]  A. Greenwald,et al.  Evidence of both perceptual filtering and response suppression for rejected messages in selective attention. , 1972, Journal of experimental psychology.

[5]  P Bertelson,et al.  Spatial Constraints on Focused Attention: Beyond the Right-Side Advantage , 1976, Perception.

[6]  Spatial Effects in Speech Perception in the Absence of Spatial Competition , 1977, Perception.

[7]  D. Shinar Psychology on the Road: The Human Factor in Traffic Safety , 1978 .

[8]  Jean Requin,et al.  Spatial constraints on attention to speech , 1978 .

[9]  A.J. Kames A study of the effects of mobile telephone use and control unit design on driving performance , 1978, IEEE Transactions on Vehicular Technology.

[10]  Christopher D. Wickens,et al.  The Structure of Attentional Resources , 1980 .

[11]  Postural determinants of frontal-position advantage in listening to speech , 1980, Perception & psychophysics.

[12]  J. G. Hollands,et al.  Engineering Psychology and Human Performance , 1984 .

[13]  R. Wade Allen,et al.  A simulator study of the safety implications of cellular mobile phone use , 1987 .

[14]  C D Wickens,et al.  Codes and Modalities in Multiple Resources: A Success and a Qualification , 1988, Human factors.

[15]  Michael A. Regan,et al.  Division of Attention: The Single-Channel Hypothesis Revisited , 1989 .

[16]  K A Brookhuis,et al.  The effects of mobile telephoning on driving performance. , 1991, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[17]  C. Wickens Engineering psychology and human performance, 2nd ed. , 1992 .

[18]  A J McKnight,et al.  The effect of cellular phone use upon driver attention. , 1993, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[19]  D R Begault,et al.  Head-Up Auditory Displays for Traffic Collision Avoidance System Advisories: A Preliminary Investigation , 1993, Human factors.

[20]  Jon Driver,et al.  Covert Spatial Orienting in Audition: Exogenous and Endogenous Mechanisms , 1994 .

[21]  Charles Spence,et al.  Spatial synergies between auditory and visual attention. , 1994 .

[22]  M. Moscovitch,et al.  Attention and Performance 15: Conscious and Nonconscious Information Processing , 1994 .

[23]  H Alm,et al.  The effects of a mobile telephone task on driver behaviour in a car following situation. , 1995, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[24]  D. Vinson,et al.  Alcohol and injury. A case-crossover study. , 1995, Archives of family medicine.

[25]  J. Driver,et al.  Audiovisual links in endogenous covert spatial attention. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[26]  J M Violanti,et al.  Cellular phones and traffic accidents: an epidemiological approach. , 1996, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[27]  Jon Driver,et al.  Cross-modal links in attention between audition, vision, and touch: Implications for interface design , 1997 .

[28]  J. Driver,et al.  Audiovisual links in exogenous covert spatial orienting , 1997, Perception & psychophysics.

[29]  John Duncan,et al.  Restricted attentional capacity within but not between sensory modalities , 1997, Nature.

[30]  R. Tibshirani,et al.  Association between cellular-telephone calls and motor vehicle collisions. , 1997, The New England journal of medicine.

[31]  H. Beh,et al.  Performance on driving-related tasks during music , 1999 .

[32]  F. McKenna,et al.  The effect of interference on dynamic risk‐taking judgments , 1999 .

[33]  Jon Driver,et al.  A new approach to the design of multimodal warning signals , 1999 .

[34]  Martin Eimer,et al.  Can attention be directed to opposite locations in different modalities? An ERP study , 1999, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[35]  Matthew P. Reed,et al.  Comparison of driving performance on-road and in a low-cost simulator using a concurrent telephone dialling task , 1999 .

[36]  R Graham,et al.  Use of auditory icons as emergency warnings: evaluation within a vehicle collision avoidance application. , 1999, Ergonomics.

[37]  A. North,et al.  Music and driving game performance , 1999 .

[38]  J Driver,et al.  Cross-modal selective attention: On the difficulty of ignoring sounds at the locus of visual attention , 2000, Perception & psychophysics.

[39]  Warren Brodsky,et al.  The effects of music tempo on simulated driving performance and vehicular control , 2001 .

[40]  S J Westerman,et al.  Mobile (cellular) phone use and driving: a critical review of research methodology , 2001, Ergonomics.

[41]  David L. Strayer,et al.  Driven to Distraction: Dual-Task Studies of Simulated Driving and Conversing on a Cellular Telephone , 2001, Psychological science.