Changes in stereotyped central motor patterns controlling vocalization are induced by peripheral nerve injury.

Adult male zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata), as closed-ended learners, normally crystallize their songs at 90 days of age, and the song remains fixed throughout life (Price, 1979). We show that injuring the tracheosyringeal nerve(s) (each of which innervates the ipsilateral half of the syrinx, the avian vocal organ) results in a short-term deficit in the syllables forming adult male song; this deficit disappears after ts nerve regeneration. However, when adult males were followed for a period of several weeks after unilateral tracheosyringeal nerve injury, long-term changes occurred in the temporal patterning of song. Syllables were deleted, remaining portions of the song were linked, and new syllables were added. Syllables with call-like morphology were less likely to be deleted from and more likely to be added to the song. Deletions were most often contiguous chunks of syllables. Changes in the temporal patterning of song occurred during specific periods following nerve injury, were completed within 100 days after nerve transection, and were not dependent upon regeneration of the ts nerve. The resulting newly formed song patterns were stable, remaining unchanged up to 1 year later. The ability of adult male zebra finches to make specific types of changes to crystallized song indicates that some form of vocal plasticity remains even after song learning is completed, though this plasticity may be restricted to a subset of song characteristics. The limitations on the types of changes that are possible may reflect how song is centrally organized.

[1]  Philip H. Price Developmental determinants of structure in zebra finch song. , 1979 .

[2]  H. Williams,et al.  Syllable chunking in zebra finch (Taeniopygia guttata) song. , 1992, Journal of comparative psychology.

[3]  H. Williams Models for song learning in the zebra finch: fathers or others? , 1990, Animal Behaviour.

[4]  H. Williams,et al.  Sexual dimorphism of auditory activity in the zebra finch song system. , 1985, Behavioral and neural biology.

[5]  F. Nottebohm A brain for all seasons: cyclical anatomical changes in song control nuclei of the canary brain. , 1981, Science.

[6]  H. Williams,et al.  Timbre control in zebra finch (Taeniopygia guttata) song syllables. , 1989, Journal of comparative psychology.

[7]  F. Nottebohm,et al.  A comparative study of the behavioral deficits following lesions of various parts of the zebra finch song system: implications for vocal learning , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[8]  B. Sumner A quantitative analysis of the response of presynaptic boutons to postsynaptic motor neuron axotomy , 1975, Experimental Neurology.

[9]  E. Nordeen,et al.  Projection neurons within a vocal motor pathway are born during song learning in zebra finches , 1988, Nature.

[10]  A. Arnold,et al.  Ontogeny of brain nuclei controlling song learning and behavior in zebra finches , 1985, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[11]  A. Arnold,et al.  Sexual dimorphism in vocal control areas of the songbird brain. , 1976, Science.

[12]  R. Sossinka,et al.  Song Types in the Zebra Finch Poephila guttata castanotis1 , 1980 .

[13]  E. Nordeen,et al.  Selective impairment of song learning following lesions of a forebrain nucleus in the juvenile zebra finch. , 1990, Behavioral and neural biology.

[14]  J. Cynx,et al.  Experimental determination of a unit of song production in the zebra finch (Taeniopygia guttata). , 1990, Journal of comparative psychology.

[15]  H. Williams,et al.  Right-side dominance for song control in the zebra finch. , 1992, Journal of neurobiology.

[16]  F. Nottebohm,et al.  Central control of song in the canary, Serinus canarius , 1976, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[17]  F. Nottebohm,et al.  Birth of projection neurons in the higher vocal center of the canary forebrain before, during, and after song learning. , 1988, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[18]  E. Nordeen,et al.  Auditory feedback is necessary for the maintenance of stereotyped song in adult zebra finches. , 1992, Behavioral and neural biology.

[19]  F. Nottebohm,et al.  Neural lateralization of vocal control in a passerine bird. I. Song. , 1971, The Journal of experimental zoology.

[20]  J. S. McCasland,et al.  Neuronal control of bird song production , 1987, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[21]  A. Arnold,et al.  Forebrain lesions disrupt development but not maintenance of song in passerine birds. , 1984, Science.

[22]  A. Arnold,et al.  The role of feedback from the vocal organ. I. Maintenance of stereotypical vocalizations by adult zebra finches , 1984, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[23]  K. Immelmann Song development in the zebra finch and other estrildid finches , 1969 .

[24]  J. Wild,et al.  A respiratory-vocal pathway in the brainstem of the pigeon , 1987, Brain Research.

[25]  A. King,et al.  Epigenesis of cowbird song—A joint endeavour of males and females , 1983, Nature.

[26]  Masakazu Konishi,et al.  Neuronal growth, atrophy and death in a sexually dimorphic song nucleus in the zebra finch brain , 1985, Nature.

[27]  Fernando Nottebohm,et al.  Migration of young neurons in adult avian brain , 1988, Nature.

[28]  D. Vicario,et al.  Brain pathways for learned and unlearned vocalizations differ in zebra finches , 1990, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.