Observation Can Be as Effective as Action in Problem Solving

This study discusses findings that replicate and extend the original work of Burns and Vollmeyer (2002), which showed that performance in problem-solving tasks was more accurate when people were engaged in a non-specific goal than in a specific goal. The main innovation here was to examine the goal specificity effect under both observation-based and conventional action-based learning conditions. The findings show that goal specificity affects the accuracy of problem solving in the same way when the learning stage of the task is observation-based as when it is action-based. In addition, the findings show that, when instructions do not promote goal specificity, observation-based problem solving is as effective as action-based problem solving.

[1]  M. Rossano Expertise and the evolution of consciousness , 2003, Cognition.

[2]  S. Sala,et al.  Retrieval of performed versus to-be-performed tasks: A naturalistic study of the intention–superiority effect in normal aging and dementia. , 2000 .

[3]  David Klahr,et al.  Dual Space Search During Scientific Reasoning , 1988, Cogn. Sci..

[4]  A. Bandura,et al.  Negative self-efficacy and goal effects revisited. , 2003, The Journal of applied psychology.

[5]  Dietrich Dörner,et al.  The Logic Of Failure: Recognizing And Avoiding Error In Complex Situations , 1997 .

[6]  W. Brogden Annual Review of Psychology , 1957 .

[7]  John Sweller,et al.  Development of expertise in mathematical problem solving , 1983 .

[8]  K. A. Ericsson,et al.  Expert and exceptional performance: evidence of maximal adaptation to task constraints. , 1996, Annual review of psychology.

[9]  Jeffrey B. Vancouver,et al.  The Application of HLM to the Analysis of the Dynamic Interaction of Environment, Person and Behavior , 1997 .

[10]  R. Glaser,et al.  Learning Theory and the Study of Instruction , 1989 .

[11]  A M Burton,et al.  Learning complex sequences: no role for observation? , 2001, Psychological research.

[12]  Dianne C. Berry,et al.  The combination of explicit and implicit learning processes in task control , 1987 .

[13]  Fiona Godlee,et al.  Testing common sense , 2007, BMJ : British Medical Journal.

[14]  E. Salas,et al.  Taking stock of naturalistic decision making , 2001 .

[15]  Gregory Schraw Promoting general metacognitive awareness , 1998 .

[16]  Gordon D. A. Brown,et al.  Scale invariance in the retrieval of retrospective and prospective memories , 2001, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[17]  E. A. Locke,et al.  Building a practically useful theory of goal setting and task motivation. A 35-year odyssey. , 2002, The American psychologist.

[18]  G. Miller,et al.  Cognitive science. , 1981, Science.

[19]  D. Broadbent,et al.  Interactive tasks and the implicit‐explicit distinction , 1988 .

[20]  J. Quesada,et al.  Cognitive flexibility and adaptability to environmental changes in dynamic complex problem-solving tasks , 2003, Ergonomics.

[21]  Herbert A. Simon,et al.  Problem solving and rule induction: A unified view. , 1974 .

[22]  Stephen K. Reed,et al.  Differences in expert and novice situation awareness in naturalistic decision making , 1996, Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud..

[23]  J. Sweller,et al.  What do students learn while solving mathematics problems , 1985 .

[24]  M. Levine,et al.  Effects of Goal Specificity on Means-Ends Analysis and Learning. , 1982 .

[25]  Y. S. Lee,et al.  Effects of learning contexts on implicit and explicit learning , 1995, Memory & cognition.

[26]  Dianne C. Berry,et al.  The Role of Action in Implicit Learning , 1991 .

[27]  S. Kay On the Nature of Expertise. , 1992 .

[28]  Lee W. Gregg,et al.  Knowledge and cognition , 1974 .

[29]  J. Funke Dynamic systems as tools for analysing human judgement , 2001 .

[30]  J. Sweller Evolution of human cognitive architecture , 2003 .

[31]  T. Connolly,et al.  Goals, strategy development and task performance: Some limits on the efficacy of goal setting , 1989 .

[32]  Johanna Helena Kerstholt The effect of information costs on strategy selection in dynamic tasks , 1996 .

[33]  L. Resnick,et al.  Mathematics and Science Learning: A New Conception , 1983, Science.

[34]  R. Stevenson,et al.  Explicit Learning of a Dynamic System with a Non-salient Pattern , 1997 .

[35]  G. A. Klein,et al.  Knowledge engineering : beyond expert systems , 1990 .

[36]  G. Gonzales,et al.  Information Technologies , 2019, Encyclopedia of Education and Information Technologies.

[37]  L. Schauble,et al.  Belief revision in children: the role of prior knowledge and strategies for generating evidence. , 1990, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[38]  D. Berry,et al.  Negative Correlations between Control Performance and Verbalizable Knowledge: Indicators for Implicit Learning in Process Control Tasks? , 1995, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[39]  D. Dörner The logic of failure. , 1990, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[40]  E. Glasersfeld Cognition, construction of knowledge, and teaching , 1989, Synthese.

[41]  A. Bandura Social cognitive theory of self-regulation☆ , 1991 .

[42]  Timothy E Goldsmith,et al.  Goal specificity and knowledge acquisition in statistics problem solving: Evidence for attentional focus , 2004, Memory & cognition.

[43]  B. Brehmer Dynamic decision making: human control of complex systems. , 1992, Acta psychologica.

[44]  Kenneth R. Koedinger,et al.  Goals and learning in microworlds , 1999, Cogn. Sci..

[45]  Z. Dienes,et al.  Implicit learning: Below the subjective threshold , 1997 .

[46]  K. Holyoak,et al.  The Impact of Goal Specificity on Strategy Use and the Acquisition of Problem Structure , 1996 .

[47]  Raanan Lipshitz,et al.  Converging themes in the study of decision making in realistic settings. , 1993 .

[48]  Z. Dienes,et al.  The role of specific instances in controlling a dynamic system , 1995 .

[49]  H A Simon,et al.  The theory of learning by doing. , 1979, Psychological review.

[50]  P. Pintrich,et al.  Motivational and self-regulated learning components of classroom academic performance. , 1990 .

[51]  Alexander Renkl,et al.  Learning from Worked-Out-Examples: A Study on Individual Differences , 1997, Cogn. Sci..

[52]  Mark Gluck,et al.  Memory and cognition , 2000, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[53]  M. Covington Goal theory, motivation, and school achievement: an integrative review. , 2000, Annual review of psychology.

[54]  B. Zimmerman Self-Regulated Learning and Academic Achievement: An Overview , 1990 .

[55]  T. Goschke,et al.  Representation of intentions: Persisting activation in memory. , 1993 .

[56]  Y. S. Lee,et al.  Transfer and retention of implicit and explicit learning. , 1996, British journal of psychology.

[57]  Julius Kuhl,et al.  Volition and Personality: Action Versus State Orientation , 1994 .

[58]  Suzanna L Penningroth Free recall of everyday retrospective and prospective memories: The intention-superiority effect is moderated by action versus state orientation and by gender , 2005, Memory.

[59]  D. Broadbent,et al.  Implicit and explicit knowledge in the control of complex systems , 1986 .

[60]  E. Glasersfeld Cognition, Construction of Knowledge, and Teaching , 1989 .

[61]  P. Karoly Mechanisms of Self-Regulation: A Systems View , 1993 .

[62]  Z Dienes,et al.  The Role of Implicit Memory in Controlling a Dynamic System , 1998, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[63]  Ron Sun,et al.  From implicit skills to explicit knowledge: a bottom-up model of skill learning , 2001, Cogn. Sci..

[64]  R. Mathews,et al.  Insight without Awareness: On the Interaction of Verbalization, Instruction and Practice in a Simulated Process Control Task , 1989 .

[65]  Penelope M. Sanderson,et al.  Verbalizable knowledge and skilled task performance: Association, dissociation, and mental models. , 1989 .

[66]  B. Burns,et al.  The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psychology Goal Specificity Effects on Hypothesis Testing in Problem Solving , 2022 .

[67]  Beau Fly Jones,et al.  Strategic Teaching and Learning: Cognitive Instruction in the Content Areas , 1987 .

[68]  John Sweller,et al.  Cognitive Load During Problem Solving: Effects on Learning , 1988, Cogn. Sci..

[69]  D. Broadbent,et al.  On the Relationship between Task Performance and Associated Verbalizable Knowledge , 1984 .

[70]  R. Marsh,et al.  Activation of Completed , Uncompleted , and Partially Completed Intentions , 1998 .