A theory of architecture for spatial abstraction

A great mystery is how the brain abstracts during the process of development. It is also unclear how motor actions alter cortical representation. The architecture theory introduced here indicates that for each cortical area, the bottom-up space and top-down space are two sources of its representation — bridge representation which embeds manifolds of both spaces into a single space. A bridge representation has the following properties (a) responses from developed neurons are relatively less sensitive to irrelevant sensory information (i.e., invariants) but are relatively more sensitive to relevant sensory information for classification (i.e., discriminants), (b) neurons form topographic cortical areas according to abstract classes. Both properties transform meaningless (iconic, pixel like) raw sensory inputs into an internal representation with abstract meanings. The most abstract area can be considered as frontal cortex (or motor area if each firing pattern of the motor represents a unique abstract class). Such a cortical representation system is neither a purely symbolic system nor a monolithic meaning system, but is iconic-abstract two-way: bottom-up attention, top-down attention and recognition are all tightly integrated and highly distributed throughout the developmental network.

[1]  S. Yantis,et al.  Selective visual attention and perceptual coherence , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[2]  Tomaso Poggio,et al.  Fast Readout of Object Identity from Macaque Inferior Temporal Cortex , 2005, Science.

[3]  Juyang Weng,et al.  Topographic Class Grouping with applications to 3D object recognition , 2008, 2008 IEEE International Joint Conference on Neural Networks (IEEE World Congress on Computational Intelligence).

[4]  Keiji Tanaka,et al.  Optical Imaging of Functional Organization in the Monkey Inferotemporal Cortex , 1996, Science.

[5]  Arne D. Ekstrom,et al.  Cellular networks underlying human spatial navigation , 2003, Nature.

[6]  E. Callaway Local circuits in primary visual cortex of the macaque monkey. , 1998, Annual review of neuroscience.

[7]  M. Alexander,et al.  Principles of Neural Science , 1981 .

[8]  E. Miller,et al.  Response to Comment on "Top-Down Versus Bottom-Up Control of Attention in the Prefrontal and Posterior Parietal Cortices" , 2007, Science.

[9]  Katherine M. Armstrong,et al.  Visuomotor Origins of Covert Spatial Attention , 2003, Neuron.

[10]  Juyang Weng,et al.  Optimal In-Place Learning and the Lobe Component Analysis , 2006, The 2006 IEEE International Joint Conference on Neural Network Proceedings.

[11]  S. Nelson,et al.  An emergent model of orientation selectivity in cat visual cortical simple cells , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[12]  Juyang Weng,et al.  Dually Optimal Neuronal Layers: Lobe Component Analysis , 2009, IEEE Transactions on Autonomous Mental Development.

[13]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  The Fusiform Face Area: A Module in Human Extrastriate Cortex Specialized for Face Perception , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[14]  Leslie G. Ungerleider Two cortical visual systems , 1982 .

[15]  J. Rauschecker Cortical processing of complex sounds , 1998, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[16]  Juyang Weng,et al.  2008 Special issue , 2008 .

[17]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Reorienting attention across the horizontal and vertical meridians: Evidence in favor of a premotor theory of attention , 1987, Neuropsychologia.

[18]  Jonathan Berger,et al.  Neural Dynamics of Event Segmentation in Music: Converging Evidence for Dissociable Ventral and Dorsal Networks , 2007, Neuron.

[19]  J. Weng,et al.  Optimal In-Place Self-Organization for Cortical Development : Limited Cells , Sparse Coding and Cortical Topography , 2006 .

[20]  S. Grossberg,et al.  A neural model of how horizontal and interlaminar connections of visual cortex develop into adult circuits that carry out perceptual grouping and learning. , 2010, Cerebral cortex.

[21]  Pieter R. Roelfsema,et al.  Attention-Gated Reinforcement Learning of Internal Representations for Classification , 2005, Neural Computation.

[22]  Suzanne Corkin,et al.  Posterior parahippocampal place learning in H.M. , 2007, Hippocampus.

[23]  David J. Field,et al.  Emergence of simple-cell receptive field properties by learning a sparse code for natural images , 1996, Nature.

[24]  K. Grill-Spector,et al.  Differential development of high-level visual cortex correlates with category-specific recognition memory , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[25]  J. O'Keefe,et al.  The hippocampus as a spatial map. Preliminary evidence from unit activity in the freely-moving rat. , 1971, Brain research.

[26]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Object vision and spatial vision: two cortical pathways , 1983, Trends in Neurosciences.

[27]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  Brain Imaging , 2020, Encyclopedia of Behavioral Medicine.

[28]  D. J. Felleman,et al.  Distributed hierarchical processing in the primate cerebral cortex. , 1991, Cerebral cortex.

[29]  M Corbetta,et al.  Frontoparietal cortical networks for directing attention and the eye to visual locations: identical, independent, or overlapping neural systems? , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[30]  D. Ansari Effects of development and enculturation on number representation in the brain , 2008, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[31]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  The fusiform face area subserves face perception, not generic within-category identification , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[32]  David J. Freedman,et al.  Categorical representation of visual stimuli in the primate prefrontal cortex. , 2001, Science.

[33]  Aaron R. Seitz,et al.  Laminar development of receptive fields, maps and columns in visual cortex: the coordinating role of the subplate. , 2003, Cerebral cortex.

[34]  M. Sur,et al.  Patterning and Plasticity of the Cerebral Cortex , 2005, Science.

[35]  Thomas Serre,et al.  Robust Object Recognition with Cortex-Like Mechanisms , 2007, IEEE Transactions on Pattern Analysis and Machine Intelligence.

[36]  Y. Yamane,et al.  Complex objects are represented in macaque inferotemporal cortex by the combination of feature columns , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[37]  Leila Reddy,et al.  Top–down biases win against focal attention in the fusiform face area , 2007, NeuroImage.

[38]  Risto Miikkulainen,et al.  Self-organization of hierarchical visual maps with feedback connections , 2006, Neurocomputing.

[39]  R. Desimone,et al.  Selective attention gates visual processing in the extrastriate cortex. , 1985, Science.