Response inhibition during perceptual decision making in humans and macaques

Response inhibition in stop signal tasks has been explained as the outcome of a race between GO and STOP processes (e.g., Logan, 1981). Response choice in two-alternative perceptual categorization tasks has been explained as the outcome of an accumulation of evidence for the alternative responses. To begin unifying these two powerful investigation frameworks, we obtained data from humans and macaque monkeys performing a stop signal task with responses guided by perceptual categorization and variable degrees of difficulty, ranging from low to high accuracy. Comparable results across species reinforced the validity of this animal model. Response times and errors increased with categorization difficulty. The probability of failing to inhibit responses on stop signal trials increased with stop signal delay, and the response times for failed stop signal trials were shorter than those for trials with no stop signal. Thus, the Logan race model could be applied to estimate the duration of the stopping process. We found that the duration of the STOP process did not vary across a wide range of discrimination accuracies. This is consistent with the functional, and possibly mechanistic, independence of choice and inhibition mechanisms.

[1]  Hans Colonius,et al.  Countermanding saccades: Evidence against independent processing of go and stop signals , 2003, Perception & psychophysics.

[2]  Richard J Krauzlis,et al.  Cancelling of pursuit and saccadic eye movements in humans and monkeys. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[3]  A. Vandierendonck,et al.  Inhibiting responses when switching: Does it matter? , 2005, Experimental psychology.

[4]  J D Schall,et al.  Dynamic dissociation of visual selection from saccade programming in frontal eye field. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[5]  G. Logan,et al.  In search of the point of no return: the control of response processes. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[6]  Birte U. Forstmann,et al.  How to measure post-error slowing: A confound and a simple solution , 2012 .

[7]  J. Schall,et al.  Countermanding saccades in macaque , 1995, Visual Neuroscience.

[8]  Robert Licht,et al.  Effects of stop signal modality, stop signal intensity and tracking method on inhibitory performance as determined by use of the stop signal paradigm. , 2005, Scandinavian journal of psychology.

[9]  R. Ratcliff,et al.  Estimating parameters of the diffusion model: Approaches to dealing with contaminant reaction times and parameter variability , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[10]  Roger Ratcliff,et al.  The Diffusion Decision Model: Theory and Data for Two-Choice Decision Tasks , 2008, Neural Computation.

[11]  Leanne Boucher,et al.  Nonindependent and nonstationary response times in stopping and stepping saccade tasks , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[12]  D. Meyer,et al.  The point of no return in choice reaction time: controlled and ballistic stages of response preparation. , 1986, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[13]  M. W. van der Molen,et al.  Developmental trends in simple and selective inhibition of compatible and incompatible responses. , 2004, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[14]  John I. Yellott,et al.  Correction for guessing in choice reaction time , 1967 .

[15]  J. Gold,et al.  Neural correlates of perceptual decision making before, during, and after decision commitment in monkey frontal eye field. , 2012, Cerebral cortex.

[16]  R. Ollman Fast guesses in choice reaction time , 1966 .

[17]  Takashi R Sato,et al.  Effects of Stimulus-Response Compatibility on Neural Selection in Frontal Eye Field , 2003, Neuron.

[18]  R. Carpenter,et al.  Countermanding saccades in humans , 1999, Vision Research.

[19]  R. Ratcliff,et al.  A comparison of macaque behavior and superior colliculus neuronal activity to predictions from models of two-choice decisions. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[20]  Jonathan D. Nelson,et al.  How Embodied Is Perceptual Decision Making? Evidence for Separate Processing of Perceptual and Motor Decisions , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[21]  J. Gold,et al.  Distinct Representations of a Perceptual Decision and the Associated Oculomotor Plan in the Monkey Lateral Intraparietal Area , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[22]  W. Notebaert,et al.  Effects of stimulus-stimulus compatibility and stimulus-response compatibility on response inhibition. , 2005, Acta psychologica.

[23]  G. Logan American Psychological Association, Inc.>n the Ability to Inhibit Simple Thoughts and Actions: I. Stop-Signal Studies of Decision and Memory , 2022 .

[24]  D. Kahneman Attention and Effort , 1973 .

[25]  Braden A. Purcell,et al.  From Salience to Saccades: Multiple-Alternative Gated Stochastic Accumulator Model of Visual Search , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[26]  G. Logan,et al.  Models of Response Inhibition in the Stop-signal and Stop-change Paradigms , 2022 .

[27]  A. Vandierendonck,et al.  The interaction between stop signal inhibition and distractor interference in the flanker and Stroop task. , 2004, Acta psychologica.

[28]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  A study of adaptive behavior: effects of age and irrelevant information on the ability to inhibit one's actions , 1999 .

[29]  Martin N. Hebart,et al.  Human visual and parietal cortex encode visual choices independent of motor plans , 2012, NeuroImage.

[30]  R. Bogacz Optimal decision-making theories: linking neurobiology with behaviour , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[31]  Timothy D. Hanks,et al.  Microstimulation of macaque area LIP affects decision-making in a motion discrimination task , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[32]  Patrick G. Bissett,et al.  Balancing Cognitive Demands: Control Adjustments in the Stop-signal Paradigm Stop-signal Paradigm Post-stop-signal Slowing , 2011 .

[33]  Frederick Verbruggen,et al.  How to Stop and Change a Response: the Role of Goal Activation in Multitasking , 2022 .

[34]  David E. Irwin,et al.  Don’t look! don’t touch! inhibitory control of eye and hand movements , 2000, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[35]  B. Bergum,et al.  Attention and performance IX , 1982 .

[36]  M. Shadlen,et al.  The effect of stimulus strength on the speed and accuracy of a perceptual decision. , 2005, Journal of vision.

[37]  P. M. A. RABBITT,et al.  Error Correction Time without External Error Signals , 1966, Nature.

[38]  Daniel Gopher,et al.  On the Economy of the Human Processing System: A Model of Multiple Capacity. , 1977 .

[39]  Richard P. Heitz,et al.  Neurally constrained modeling of perceptual decision making. , 2010, Psychological review.

[40]  J. Mattingley,et al.  Dissociable mechanisms of cognitive control in prefrontal and premotor cortex. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[41]  P M Rabbitt,et al.  Error-Detection and Correction Latencies as a Function of S-R Compatibility , 1967, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[42]  G. Logan On the ability to inhibit thought and action , 1984 .

[43]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Response of Neurons in the Lateral Intraparietal Area during a Combined Visual Discrimination Reaction Time Task , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[44]  N. P. Bichot,et al.  Perceptual and motor processing stages identified in the activity of macaque frontal eye field neurons during visual search. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[45]  J. Gold,et al.  The neural basis of decision making. , 2007, Annual review of neuroscience.

[46]  Aditya Murthy,et al.  Neural control of visual search by frontal eye field: effects of unexpected target displacement on visual selection and saccade preparation. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[47]  Hans Colonius,et al.  Countermanding saccades with auditory stop signals: testing the race model , 2001, Vision Research.

[48]  Frederick Verbruggen,et al.  The effect of interference in the early processing stages on response inhibition in the stop signal task , 2006, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[49]  J. Schall,et al.  Role of frontal eye fields in countermanding saccades: visual, movement, and fixation activity. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[50]  G. Logan,et al.  On the ability to inhibit simple and choice reaction time responses: a model and a method. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[51]  D. P. Hanes,et al.  Controlled Movement Processing: Superior Colliculus Activity Associated with Countermanded Saccades , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[52]  D. Munoz,et al.  Control of saccade initiation in a countermanding task using visual and auditory stop signals , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[53]  Nancy B. Carlisle,et al.  Event-related potentials elicited by errors during the stop-signal task. II: human effector-specific error responses. , 2012, Journal of neurophysiology.

[54]  S. Kelly,et al.  A supramodal accumulation-to-bound signal that determines perceptual decisions in humans , 2012, Nature Neuroscience.

[55]  Saul Sternberg,et al.  The discovery of processing stages: Extensions of Donders' method , 1969 .

[56]  G. D. Logan,et al.  Dynamics of saccade target selection: Race model analysis of double step and search step saccade production in human and macaque , 2007, Vision Research.

[57]  G. Logan,et al.  Are hyperactive children deficient in attentional capacity? , 1990, Journal of abnormal child psychology.

[58]  D Laming,et al.  Autocorrelation of choice-reaction times. , 1979, Acta psychologica.

[59]  Gordon D. Logan,et al.  On the ability to inhibit simple thoughts and actions: II. Stop-signal studies of repetition priming. , 1985 .

[60]  F T Smulders,et al.  The temporal selectivity of additive factor effects on the reaction process revealed in ERP component latencies. , 1995, Acta psychologica.

[61]  Leanne Boucher,et al.  Influence of history on saccade countermanding performance in humans and macaque monkeys , 2007, Vision Research.

[62]  R. Marois,et al.  Capacity limits of information processing in the brain , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[63]  Gordon D. Logan,et al.  Dependence and independence in responding to double stimulation: A comparison of stop, change, and dual-task paradigms. , 1986 .

[64]  Hans Colonius,et al.  Visual-tactile saccadic inhibition , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[65]  Philip L. Smith,et al.  An integrated theory of attention and decision making in visual signal detection. , 2009, Psychological review.

[66]  G. Logan,et al.  Inhibitory control in mind and brain: an interactive race model of countermanding saccades. , 2007, Psychological review.

[67]  J. Schall,et al.  Current advances and pressing problems in studies of stopping , 2012, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.