Effect of age on detecting a loss of balance in a seated whole-body balancing task.

BACKGROUND Most falls are attributed to a loss of balance without a quantitative definition of the term. It has been proposed that a loss of balance is detectable as an unusually large (anomalous) value of the system control error. The hypotheses were tested that age will not affect the detection of control error anomaly, or prediction of the associated compensatory response, in a challenging balancing task. METHODS Twenty healthy older adults were asked to sit and balance a chair over its rear legs for as long as possible. The dominant foot's ground reaction force and the chair's sagittal-plane acceleration represented the system input and output, respectively. Control error was the difference between actual and expected acceleration output from a self-identified forward internal model of the system. A control error anomaly was detected once the error crossed a threshold set at three standard deviations (3-Sigma) above the mean of baseline data. Results from five trials were compared to published results in 20 healthy young adults. FINDINGs. A control error anomaly was successfully detected in 91% of 91 older adult trials, statistically similar to the 92% success rate obtained previously in young adults. A response was predicted in 57% of the 77 older adult trials with responses, significantly less than the 92% obtained in the young adult trials (age effect significant: P<0.005). INTERPRETATION The condition leading to uncontrolled backward acceleration of the chair was reliably detected in both groups. While the young waited to respond to this condition, older subjects responded prematurely.

[1]  B E Maki,et al.  Age-related changes in compensatory stepping in response to unpredictable perturbations. , 1996, The journals of gerontology. Series A, Biological sciences and medical sciences.

[2]  B. E. Maki,et al.  The role of limb movements in maintaining upright stance: the "change-in-support" strategy. , 1997, Physical therapy.

[3]  A.D. Kuo,et al.  An optimal control model for analyzing human postural balance , 1995, IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering.

[4]  R. Enoka,et al.  Steadiness is reduced and motor unit discharge is more variable in old adults , 2000, Muscle & nerve.

[5]  L G Carlton,et al.  Old adults exhibit greater motor output variability than young adults only during rapid discrete isometric contractions. , 2001, The journals of gerontology. Series A, Biological sciences and medical sciences.

[6]  James A Ashton-Miller,et al.  Is a "loss of balance" a control error signal anomaly? Evidence for three-sigma failure detection in young adults. , 2004, Gait & posture.

[7]  J. Callicott,et al.  Neurophysiological correlates of age-related changes in human motor function , 2002, Neurology.

[8]  Effects of age on balance assessment using voluntary and involuntary step tasks. , 1999, The journals of gerontology. Series A, Biological sciences and medical sciences.

[9]  G. Wu,et al.  Distinguishing fall activities from normal activities by velocity characteristics. , 2000, Journal of biomechanics.

[10]  R. Enoka,et al.  Fluctuations in acceleration during voluntary contractions lead to greater impairment of movement accuracy in old adults. , 2003, Journal of applied physiology.

[11]  Marjorie H. Woollacott,et al.  Compensatory Stepping: The Biomechanics of a Preferred Response Among Older Adults , 2001, Experimental aging research.

[12]  Saumil N. Merchant,et al.  Temporal Bone Studies of the Human Peripheral Vestibular System , 2000, The Annals of otology, rhinology & laryngology. Supplement.

[13]  H. Skinner,et al.  Effect of articular disease and total knee arthroplasty on knee joint-position sense. , 1983, Journal of neurophysiology.

[14]  R. Anderson,et al.  Peripheral resolution using stationary and flickering gratings: the effects of age. , 1997, Current eye research.

[15]  A. Schultz,et al.  Stepping Responses of Young and Old Adults to Postural Disturbances: Kinematics , 1994, Journal of the American Geriatrics Society.

[16]  P G Wiles,et al.  Vibration Perception Threshold: Influence of Age, Height, Sex, and Smoking, and Calculation of Accurate Centile Values , 1991, Diabetic medicine : a journal of the British Diabetic Association.

[17]  B. Bergström Morphology of the vestibular nerve. II. The number of myelinated vestibular nerve fibers in man at various ages. , 1973, Acta oto-laryngologica.

[18]  V. Gusev,et al.  A model for optimal processing of multisensory information in the system for maintaining body orientation in the human , 1992, Biological Cybernetics.

[19]  M. Woollacott,et al.  Attentional demands and postural recovery: the effects of aging. , 1999, The journals of gerontology. Series A, Biological sciences and medical sciences.

[20]  J. Demer,et al.  Effect of aging on the human initial interaural linear vestibulo-ocular reflex , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[21]  G E Stelmach,et al.  Postural sway characteristics of the elderly under normal and altered visual and support surface conditions. , 1991, Journal of gerontology.

[22]  M. Béné,et al.  Influence of visual control, conduction, and central integration on static and dynamic balance in healthy older adults. , 1997, Gerontology.

[23]  L. Nashner A model describing vestibular detection of body sway motion. , 1971, Acta oto-laryngologica.

[24]  A B Schultz,et al.  Association of age with the threshold for detecting ankle inversion and eversion in upright stance. , 1995, Age and ageing.

[25]  G S Rubin,et al.  A comprehensive assessment of visual impairment in a population of older Americans. The SEE Study. Salisbury Eye Evaluation Project. , 1997, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[26]  Herman van der Kooij,et al.  A multisensory integration model of human stance control , 1999, Biological Cybernetics.