Sleep sharpens sensory stimulus coding in human visual cortex after fear conditioning

Efficient perceptual identification of emotionally-relevant stimuli requires optimized neural coding. Because sleep contributes to neural plasticity mechanisms, we asked whether the perceptual representation of emotionally-relevant stimuli within sensory cortices is modified after a period of sleep. We show combined effects of sleep and aversive conditioning on subsequent discrimination of face identity information, with parallel plasticity in the amygdala and visual cortex. After one night of sleep (but neither immediately nor after an equal waking interval), a fear-conditioned face was better detected when morphed with another identity. This behavioral change was accompanied by increased selectivity of the amygdala and face-responsive fusiform regions. Overnight neural changes can thus sharpen the representation of threat-related stimuli in cortical sensory areas, in order to improve detection in impoverished or ambiguous situations. These findings reveal an important role of sleep in shaping cortical selectivity to emotionally-relevant cues and thus promoting adaptive responses to new dangers.

[1]  P. Vuilleumier,et al.  How brains beware: neural mechanisms of emotional attention , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[2]  K. Grill-Spector,et al.  Repetition and the brain: neural models of stimulus-specific effects , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[3]  M. Walker,et al.  REM Sleep Depotentiates Amygdala Activity to Previous Emotional Experiences , 2011, Current Biology.

[4]  Christophe Phillips,et al.  Memory reactivation during rapid eye movement sleep promotes its generalization and integration in cortical stores. , 2014, Sleep.

[5]  Stefan J. Kiebel,et al.  Amygdala damage affects event‐related potentials for fearful faces at specific time windows , 2009, Human brain mapping.

[6]  A. Treves,et al.  Morphing Marilyn into Maggie dissociates physical and identity face representations in the brain , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[7]  Daniel Aeschbach,et al.  A Role for Non-Rapid-Eye-Movement Sleep Homeostasis in Perceptual Learning , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[8]  D. Paré,et al.  The Bed Nucleus of the Stria Terminalis Mediates Inter-individual Variations in Anxiety and Fear , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[9]  Robert Stickgold,et al.  Sleep promotes generalization of extinction of conditioned fear. , 2009, Sleep.

[10]  Anna Schubö,et al.  Neural evidence for the threat detection advantage: differential attention allocation to angry and happy faces. , 2011, Psychophysiology.

[11]  R. Henson,et al.  Multiple levels of visual object constancy revealed by event-related fMRI of repetition priming , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[12]  F. Keil,et al.  Categorical effects in the perception of faces , 1995, Cognition.

[13]  R. Buckner,et al.  REM Sleep, Prefrontal Theta, and the Consolidation of Human Emotional Memory , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[14]  J. Horne,et al.  A self-assessment questionnaire to determine morningness-eveningness in human circadian rhythms. , 1976, International journal of chronobiology.

[15]  E. Phelps Emotion and cognition: insights from studies of the human amygdala. , 2006, Annual review of psychology.

[16]  Peyman Golshani,et al.  Associative Fear Learning Enhances Sparse Network Coding in Primary Sensory Cortex , 2012, Neuron.

[17]  Christophe Phillips,et al.  The Impact of Visual Perceptual Learning on Sleep and Local Slow-Wave Initiation , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[18]  P. Maquet,et al.  Neural correlates of perceptual learning: A functional MRI study of visual texture discrimination , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[19]  Kalanit Grill-Spector,et al.  Sparsely-distributed organization of face and limb activations in human ventral temporal cortex , 2010, NeuroImage.

[20]  C. H. Hansen,et al.  Finding the face in the crowd: an anger superiority effect. , 1988, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[21]  Dieter Wallach,et al.  Circadian modulation of sequence learning under high and low sleep pressure conditions , 2004, Behavioural Brain Research.

[22]  R. Dolan,et al.  Effects of Attention and Emotion on Face Processing in the Human Brain An Event-Related fMRI Study , 2001, Neuron.

[23]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[24]  J. Hobson,et al.  The Role of Sleep in Learning and Memory , 2014 .

[25]  J. Hobson,et al.  Visual discrimination learning requires sleep after training , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[26]  T. Shallice,et al.  Neuroimaging evidence for dissociable forms of repetition priming. , 2000, Science.

[27]  T. Parrish,et al.  Aversive Learning Enhances Perceptual and Cortical Discrimination of Indiscriminable Odor Cues , 2008, Science.

[28]  M. Desseilles,et al.  Sleep Promotes the Neural Reorganization of Remote Emotional Memory , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[29]  J. Born,et al.  Early sleep triggers memory for early visual discrimination skills , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[30]  Manuel Schabus,et al.  Sleep-Related Hippocampo-Cortical Interplay during Emotional Memory Recollection , 2007, PLoS biology.

[31]  Z. Kourtzi,et al.  Neural coding of global form in the human visual cortex. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[32]  James L. McGaugh,et al.  The basolateral amygdala modulates specific sensory memory representations in the cerebral cortex , 2009, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[33]  A. Ohman,et al.  The face in the crowd revisited: a threat advantage with schematic stimuli. , 2001, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[34]  Matthew P Walker,et al.  Sleep Facilitates Consolidation of Emotional Declarative Memory , 2006, Psychological science.

[35]  N. Weinberger Associative representational plasticity in the auditory cortex: a synthesis of two disciplines. , 2007, Learning & memory.

[36]  Joseph E LeDoux,et al.  Stimulus generalization of fear responses: effects of auditory cortex lesions in a computational model and in rats. , 1997, Cerebral cortex.

[37]  E. Fischer Conditioned Reflexes , 1942, American journal of physical medicine.

[38]  Elizabeth A. Kensinger,et al.  Sleep Leads to Changes in the Emotional Memory Trace: Evidence from fMRI , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[39]  Rony Paz,et al.  Auditory aversive learning increases discrimination thresholds , 2011, Nature Neuroscience.

[40]  Joseph E LeDoux,et al.  Contributions of the Amygdala to Emotion Processing: From Animal Models to Human Behavior , 2005, Neuron.

[41]  C. Spielberger,et al.  Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory , 1970 .

[42]  R. Dolan,et al.  Distant influences of amygdala lesion on visual cortical activation during emotional face processing , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[43]  Andreas Lüthi,et al.  Generalization of amygdala LTP and conditioned fear in the absence of presynaptic inhibition , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[44]  S. Ghirlanda,et al.  A century of generalization , 2003, Animal Behaviour.

[45]  J. Born,et al.  Emotional memory formation is enhanced across sleep intervals with high amounts of rapid eye movement sleep. , 2001, Learning & memory.

[46]  R. Spencer,et al.  Processing of Emotional Reactivity and Emotional Memory over Sleep , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[47]  Julia S. Rihm,et al.  The role of sleep and sleep deprivation in consolidating fear memories , 2013, NeuroImage.

[48]  N. Weinberger Specific long-term memory traces in primary auditory cortex , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[49]  M. Johns,et al.  A new method for measuring daytime sleepiness: the Epworth sleepiness scale. , 1991, Sleep.

[50]  C. Grillon,et al.  Overgeneralization of conditioned fear as a pathogenic marker of panic disorder. , 2010, The American journal of psychiatry.

[51]  David F. Dinges,et al.  Microcomputer analyses of performance on a portable, simple visual RT task during sustained operations , 1985 .

[52]  J. Driver,et al.  Modulation of visual processing by attention and emotion: windows on causal interactions between human brain regions , 2007, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[53]  Robert Stickgold,et al.  Sleep Preferentially Enhances Memory for Emotional Components of Scenes , 2008, Psychological science.

[54]  James L. McGaugh,et al.  Activation of the basolateral amygdala induces long-term enhancement of specific memory representations in the cerebral cortex , 2013, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[55]  R. Cabeza,et al.  Cognitive neuroscience of emotional memory , 2006, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[56]  Takeo Watanabe,et al.  Location-Specific Cortical Activation Changes during Sleep after Training for Perceptual Learning , 2009, Current Biology.

[57]  R A Steer,et al.  Further Evidence for the Construct Validity of the Beck Depression Inventory-II with Psychiatric Outpatients , 1997, Psychological reports.

[58]  B W Ellis,et al.  The St. Mary's Hospital sleep questionnaire: a study of reliability. , 1981, Sleep.

[59]  Luiz Pessoa,et al.  Affective learning increases sensitivity to graded emotional faces. , 2008, Emotion.

[60]  Daniel J Buysse,et al.  The Pittsburgh sleep quality index: A new instrument for psychiatric practice and research , 1989, Psychiatry Research.

[61]  J. Born,et al.  The memory function of sleep , 2010, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[62]  T. Åkerstedt,et al.  Subjective and objective sleepiness in the active individual. , 1990, The International journal of neuroscience.

[63]  R. Stickgold,et al.  Sleep-dependent memory triage: evolving generalization through selective processing , 2013, Nature Neuroscience.

[64]  R. Chervin Epworth sleepiness scale? , 2003, Sleep medicine.