Impulsivity and strategy transfer: metamemory as mediator.

Acquisition, maintenance, and generalization of organizational strategies were studied as a function of impulsivity-reflectivity and metamemory. In Experiment 1, 64 second- and third-grade children, identified as impulsive or reflective, participated in strategy training and transfer sessions; 25 children in a control condition received the same tasks without strategy training. Strategy and metamemory scores were significantly higher for reflective than impulsive children. Metamemory predicted strategic behavior when cognitive tempo was removed. In Experiment 2, 80 children were given an expanded metamemory battery designed to assess verbalized knowledge of memory processes. Children in the experimental condition were trained to use an organizational strategy and later tested for its maintenance and transfer. Strategy scores were higher for the reflective children during transfer but not training, suggesting a relationship between cognitive tempo and the ability to use strategies in new contexts. Reflective children were again found to have higher metamemory scores. The correlation between metamemory and strategy use with cognitive tempo partialed out was significant, whereas the tempo-strategy use correlation was nonsignificant when metamemory was removed. Strategy maintenance and generalization in impulsive and reflective children appear mediated by metamemorial processes.

[1]  J. Borkowski,et al.  On the reliability and validity of children’s metamemory , 1982 .

[2]  J. Borkowski,et al.  Metamemory and the transfer of an interrogative strategy by EMR children , 1980 .

[3]  B A Ringel,et al.  On knowing how well one is remembering: the persistence of strategy use during transfer. , 1980, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[4]  R. Ault,et al.  Reflection-impulsivity and the evaluation process. , 1979, Child development.

[5]  D. Meichenbaum,et al.  Verbal Rehearsal and Serial Recall: The Mediational Training of Kindergarten Children. , 1979 .

[6]  J. Borkowski,et al.  The Metamemory-Memory “Connection”: Effects of Strategy Training and Maintenance , 1979 .

[7]  Tommy Cammock,et al.  Development of a More Reliable Version of the Matching Familiar Figures Test. , 1978 .

[8]  John E. Readence,et al.  Modification of Impulsive Cognitive Style: A Survey of the Literature , 1978, Psychological reports.

[9]  J. Birkimer,et al.  Role of self-instruction and self-reinforcement in the modification of impulsivity. , 1978 .

[10]  M. Pressley,et al.  Developmental Changes in Assessing Recall and Recognition Memory Capacity. , 1977 .

[11]  T. Gruenenfelder,et al.  Transfer of Mediational Strategies in Children: The Role of Activity and Awareness During Strategy Acquisition. , 1976 .

[12]  T. Cooper Department of Health, Education, and Welfare. , 1976, Military medicine.

[13]  Beth Ann Kennedy,et al.  Persistent use of verbal rehearsal as a function of information about its value. , 1976 .

[14]  N. N. Bender Self-Verbalization Versus Tutor Verbalization in Modifying Impulsivity. , 1976 .

[15]  J. Mckinney Development of Problem Solving Strategies in Reflective and Impulsive Children. , 1975 .

[16]  J. Flavell,et al.  An interview study of children's knowledge about memory , 1975 .

[17]  A. Siegel,et al.  Recognition memory in reflective and impulsive preschool children. , 1973, Child development.

[18]  R. Ault Problem-solving strategies of reflective, impulsive, fast-accurate, and slow-inaccurate children. , 1973, Child development.

[19]  Sam C. Brown,et al.  Comparison of measures for the estimation of clustering in free recall. , 1971 .

[20]  D. Meichenbaum,et al.  Training impulsive children to talk to themselves: a means of developing self-control. , 1971, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[21]  E. Siegelman,et al.  Reflective and impulsive observing behavior. , 1969, Child development.

[22]  J. Kagan,et al.  Modifiability of an impulsive tempo. , 1966, Journal of educational psychology.

[23]  J. Kagan,et al.  Conceptual impulsivity and inductive reasoning. , 1966, Child development.

[24]  J. Kagan Reflection--impulsivity: the generality and dynamics of conceptual tempo. , 1966, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[25]  R. Sperber,et al.  Learning and cognition in the mentally retarded , 1984 .

[26]  J. P. Das,et al.  Intelligence and learning , 1981 .

[27]  E. Butterfield Testing Process Theories of Intelligence , 1981 .

[28]  John G. Borkowski,et al.  Searching for Metamemory-Memory Connections: A Developmental Study , 1980 .

[29]  J. Flavell Metacognition and Cognitive Monitoring: A New Area of Cognitive-Developmental Inquiry. , 1979 .

[30]  W. E. Jeffrey,et al.  Reflective and impulsive children: strategies of information processing underlying differences in problem solving. , 1976, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[31]  J. Mckinney Problem-Solving Strategies in Impulsive and Reflective Second Graders. , 1973 .

[32]  D. Massari,et al.  Discrimination Learning by Reflective and Impulsive Children as a Function of Reinforcement Schedule. , 1972 .

[33]  D. Drake,et al.  Perceptual correlates of impulsive and reflective behavior. , 1970 .