Health Decision Aids To Facilitate Shared Decision Making in Office Practice

In the day-to-day practice of medicine, patients and their physicians face an increasing number of complex choices regarding prevention, diagnosis, and treatment. The options usually have different profiles of potential benefits and risks (not to mention costs). Deber and colleagues (1, 2) have thoughtfully distinguished problem solving tasks in medicine, which involve one right course of action and provide little room for patient involvement, and decision-making tasks, in which there are several reasonable courses of action and patient involvement is important (1, 2). The optimal choice for a decision-making task depends not only on the probabilities of various outcomes with each strategy but also on the patient's relative preferences for the possible outcome states (and their timing) and his or her attitudes toward risk (3, 4). Health Decision Aids Health decision aids are designed to facilitate shared decision making by helping patients and their physicians choose among reasonable clinical options (5, 6). Although these aids vary widely in content, common denominators are the presentation of more than one reasonable strategy for a clinical management question and a description of the possible outcomes of the various options. Other components may include some form of vicarious experience with possible outcomes (through written or videotaped testimonies from former patients) or exercises designed to help clarify patients' values as they pertain to the decision at hand. More complicated, interactive decision aids allow patients to take an active part in determining the amount and type of the information they receive about their health problem. In general, decision aids are meant to supplement, not replace, the traditional process of patient counseling by clinicians. Although decision aids generally present a menu of standard management options, they have also been used to present the additional option of a randomized clinical trial for informed patients who are at personal equipoise regarding two treatment options (7, 8). Health decision aids take many forms. The most common are combinations of written and oral information (including audiotapes); personal counseling, sometimes supplemented by decision boards; linear videotapes; and interactive, computer-driven multimedia programs. Randomized trials have examined the effectiveness of each of these types of decision aids, addressing a variety of medical decisions. Of interest, although simple written brochures are probably the most widely used decision aids in office practice, they are the least well studied. Measuring a Good Decision Later in this paper I discuss the evidence on the impact of health decision aids. First, however, it is important to be clear about how one would expect such programs to affect patients and the process and outcomes of medical decision making. Several recent reviews have addressed this topic (9, 10). In the big picture, use of health decision aids should result in better decisions. But how does one measure a good decision? Commonly measured outcomes in trials of decision aids include patients' knowledge about the condition of interest and its management options, their satisfaction with the process of decision making or the decision, their perceived level of participation in the decision-making process, their level of conflict about the decision, the treatment choice actually selected, and overall or disease-specific health status. If the choice of a screening or treatment strategy is to be truly informed, patient knowledge about the decision seems a straightforward outcome measure. Investigators studying the impact of decision aids have usually used short, problem-specific questionnaires to assess the impact of decision aids on patient knowledge. Satisfaction with the decision-making process and the decision itself can be measured by using validated instruments (11, 12). Participation in decision making is commonly assessed by using a single-item (13) or multi-item scale (14), the Autonomy Preference Index (14). The degree of conflict a patient feels about a decision is often measured by using the Decisional Conflict Scale (5). Choice of treatment is also decision specific, but changes in what patients elect to do alone do not indicate whether the therapeutic choices are better with a decision aid. Another logical end point for choice of strategy is whether decision aids result in selections that are more consistent with a patient's assessed outcome preferences, risk attitudes, and time preferences. Finally, improved decision making may lead to better health status in some situations, which can be measured by using a host of disease-specific or generic health status measurements. Of note, older research with coaching interventions used to encourage patient participation at their office visits has suggested that increasing patients' participation in their care appears to improve health status, independent of effects on their diseases (15, 16). More recently, we found similar results in a trial of a decision aid for men facing a decision about treatment for benign prostatic hyperplasia (12). There is no clear consensus on which combination of these measures best assesses the impact of a decision aid. Other attributes, such as level of patient anxiety and trust in the physician, may be important as well. I believe that measurements of knowledge, satisfaction with the decision-making process, choice of management strategy, and decisional conflict form a core set of outcome measures for trials of health decision aids. Ideally, trials should also assess patients' preferences for the important outcomes of the candidate strategies (and perhaps risk attitudes and time preferences as well) to determine whether exposure to the decision aid indeed better tailors management choices to patient preferences. The Thorny Issue of Balance When patients face medical decisions with multiple options, there may be many stakeholders. Manufacturers of screening tests, pharmaceutical agents, and medical devices may have a financial interest in promoting a particular choice, as may different specialty groups. Financial conflicts aside, different parties may strongly feel that one particular course of action is right. Recent debates about the value of screening mammography for women in their 40s (17-19) or the value of prostate-specific antigen (PSA) screening (20-23) demonstrate these passions in action. Obviously, decision aids should be developed to present the risks and benefits of candidate options in a nonbiased way; but bias, like beauty, is in the eye of the beholder. Avoiding real or apparent financial conflicts of interest in the development of decision aids and obtaining assessments of balance from viewers (patients and physicians without an obvious stake in the management decision) are two approaches for minimizing bias while developing decision aids. However, ratings of balance by viewers can be tricky. For example, my colleagues and I have shown that although men with benign prostatic hyperplasia rated a decision aid balanced overall, men leaning toward surgery rated the program as being somewhat more supportive of surgery whereas men leaning toward nonsurgical treatment rated the program as being somewhat more supportive of avoiding surgery (24). Effectiveness of Health Decision Aids Two recent systematic reviews have described the results of studies of health decision aids. O'Connor and colleagues (25) conducted a Cochrane systematic review of randomized trials of health decision aids, while Molenaar and colleagues (26) examined both noncontrolled and controlled studies (26). O'Connor and colleagues' review covered trials published through early 1998. Only randomized, controlled trials comparing decision aids to controls or alternative interventions were included. Patients had to be facing actual management decisions, and the decision aids being evaluated had to include, at a minimum, information on options and outcomes relevant to the user's health. The authors identified 17 trials addressing 11 screening or treatment decisions. One outcome measured in 8 trials was patient knowledge about the condition of interest and its management options (Table 1). Four trials comparing a decision aid against usual care showed a significant improvement in patients' knowledge; the weighted mean difference in scores was 19 points (95% CI, 13 to 25 points) on a scale of 0 (none correct) to 100 (all correct). Four trials comparing a more intensive with a less intensive decision aid found a more modest (but still significant) improvement with the more complex program, with a weighted mean difference of 3 points (CI, 0.7 to 5 points). The clinical significance of a difference of this magnitude is uncertain. Decision aids also positively affected decisional conflict in 3 of 4 trials measuring that outcome, with a weighted mean difference of 0.3 (CI, 0.1 to 0.4) on a 0 to 5 scale. Only 1 of 5 studies measuring some aspect of patient satisfaction showed significant improvement with a decision aid (on satisfaction with the decision-making process), and the overall measure of effect was not statistically significant. Table 1. Effect of Decision Aids on Patients' Knowledge of Options and Outcomes Fourteen trials examined the impact of a decision aid on the choice of a management strategy (Table 2). Here, the results depended on the type of decision. For major surgery, decision aids led to more conservative therapeutic choices, with 21% to 42% reductions in selection of the most invasive treatment option and an overall relative risk for choosing the most invasive option of 0.74 (CI, 0.6 to 0.9). No significant effect was seen with decision aids for circumcision, BRCA1 gene screening, amniocentesis, or hormone replacement therapy. One study found a significant increase in hepatitis B vaccination with exposure to a decision aid. Three included trials addressing PSA scree

[1]  S H Kaplan,et al.  Patients' participation in medical care: effects on blood sugar control and quality of life in diabetes. , 1988, Journal of general internal medicine.

[2]  R. Volk,et al.  Decision-aids for prostate cancer screening. , 2000, The Journal of family practice.

[3]  W. Levinson,et al.  Informed decision making in outpatient practice: time to get back to basics. , 1999, JAMA.

[4]  M. Holmes-Rovner,et al.  An educational intervention as decision support for menopausal women. , 1997, Research in nursing & health.

[5]  Stephan D. Fihn,et al.  How Doctors and Patients Discuss Routine Clinical Decisions: Informed Decision Making in the Outpatient Setting , 1997 .

[6]  M. Weinstein,et al.  Clinical Decision Analysis , 1980 .

[7]  H. Llewellyn-Thomas,et al.  Patients' Health-care Decision Making , 1995, Medical decision making : an international journal of the Society for Medical Decision Making.

[8]  J. Benkendorf,et al.  Controlled trial of pretest education approaches to enhance informed decision-making for BRCA1 gene testing. , 1997, Journal of the National Cancer Institute.

[9]  A. O'Connor Validation of a Decisional Conflict Scale , 1995, Medical decision making : an international journal of the Society for Medical Decision Making.

[10]  M. Holmes-Rovner,et al.  Patient Satisfaction with Health Care Decisions , 1996, Medical decision making : an international journal of the Society for Medical Decision Making.

[11]  R. Volk,et al.  A randomized controlled trial of shared decision making for prostate cancer screening. , 1999, Archives of family medicine.

[12]  R. Street,et al.  Increasing patient involvement in choosing treatment for early breast cancer , 1995, Cancer.

[13]  M. Starling,et al.  A randomized controlled trial of information‐giving to patients referred for coronary angiography: effects on outcomes of care , 1998, Health expectations : an international journal of public participation in health care and health policy.

[14]  V. Entwistle,et al.  Evaluating Interventions to Promote Patient Involvement in Decision-Making: By What Criteria Should Effectiveness be Judged? , 1998, Journal of health services research & policy.

[15]  S. Fletcher,et al.  Breast cancer screening among women in their forties: an overview of the issues. , 1997, Journal of the National Cancer Institute. Monographs.

[16]  R. Deber,et al.  What role do patients wish to play in treatment decision making? , 1996, Archives of internal medicine.

[17]  B. Lo,et al.  Do patients want to participate in medical decision making? , 1984, JAMA.

[18]  D. Kopans The breast cancer screening controversy and the National Institutes of Health Consensus Development Conference on Breast Cancer Screening for Women Ages 40-49. , 1999, Radiology.

[19]  Susan Michie,et al.  Patient decision making: An evaluation of two different methods of presenting information about a screening test , 1997 .

[20]  John E. Ware,et al.  Expanding Patient Involvement in Care , 1985 .

[21]  H. D. de Koning,et al.  PSA screening for prostate cancer: the current controversy. , 1998, Annals of oncology : official journal of the European Society for Medical Oncology.

[22]  M. J. Maisels,et al.  Circumcision: The Effect of Information on Parental Decision Making , 1983, Pediatrics.

[23]  A. Herrera,et al.  Parental information and circumcision in highly motivated couples with higher education. , 1983, Pediatrics.

[24]  F Davidoff,et al.  Important Elements of Outpatient Care: A Comparison of Patients' and Physicians' Opinions , 1996, Annals of Internal Medicine.

[25]  M. Barry,et al.  Indications for treatment of benign prostatic hyperplasia. The American Urological Association Study , 1992, Cancer.

[26]  M. Barry,et al.  PSA screening for prostate cancer: the current controversy--a viewpoint. Patient Outcomes Research Team for Prostatic Diseases. , 1998, Annals of oncology : official journal of the European Society for Medical Oncology.

[27]  J. Schorling,et al.  The impact of informed consent on patient interest in prostate-specific antigen screening. , 1996, Archives of internal medicine.

[28]  V. Entwistle,et al.  Decision aids for patients facing health treatment or screening decisions: systematic review , 1999, BMJ.

[29]  T. Wilt,et al.  The prostate cancer intervention versus observation trial (PIVOT). A randomized trial comparing radical prostatectomy versus expectant management for the treatment of clinically localized prostate cancer , 1995, Oncology.

[30]  Sankey V. Williams,et al.  Guiding individual decisions: a randomized, controlled trial of decision analysis. , 1988, The American journal of medicine.

[31]  S. Fletcher,et al.  Whither scientific deliberation in health policy recommendations? Alice in the Wonderland of breast-cancer screening. , 1997, The New England journal of medicine.

[32]  R. Deber,et al.  Physicians in health care management: 8. The patient-physician partnership: decision making, problem solving and the desire to participate. , 1994, CMAJ : Canadian Medical Association journal = journal de l'Association medicale canadienne.

[33]  G. Elwyn,et al.  How Should Effectiveness of Risk Communication to Aid Patients' Decisions Be Judged? , 1999, Medical decision making : an international journal of the Society for Medical Decision Making.

[34]  I. Thompson,et al.  PSA screening--current controversy. , 1998, Annals of oncology : official journal of the European Society for Medical Oncology.

[35]  A. Ash,et al.  Measuring patients' desire for autonomy: decision making and information-seeking preferences among medical patients. , 1989, Journal of general internal medicine.

[36]  C. Phillips,et al.  The influence of video imaging on patients' perceptions and expectations. , 1995, The Angle orthodontist.

[37]  Floyd J. Fowler,et al.  A Randomized Trial of a Multimedia Shared Decision-Making Program for Men Facing a Treatment Decision for Benign Prostatic Hyperplasia , 1997 .

[38]  T. Hassard,et al.  Information and patient participation in screening for prostate cancer. , 1999, Patient education and counseling.

[39]  M. Barry,et al.  Patient Reactions to a Program Designed to Facilitate Patient Participation in Treatment Decisions for Benign Prostatic Hyperplasia , 1995, Medical care.

[40]  R. Volk,et al.  Screening for prostate cancer with the prostate-specific antigen test: are patients making informed decisions? , 1999, The Journal of family practice.

[41]  S. Kaplan,et al.  Expanding patient involvement in care. Effects on patient outcomes. , 1985, Annals of internal medicine.

[42]  Jenny Donovan,et al.  Screening for prostate cancer , 1993, JAMA.

[43]  E. Rutgers,et al.  Interpretive Review : Feasibility and Effects of Decision Aids , 2000, Medical decision making : an international journal of the Society for Medical Decision Making.

[44]  D. Sulmasy,et al.  What should men know about prostate-specific antigen screening before giving informed consent? , 1998, The American journal of medicine.

[45]  W. Levinson,et al.  Communication between surgeons and patients in routine office visits. , 1999, Surgery.

[46]  M. Schapira,et al.  The effect of an illustrated pamphlet decision-aid on the use of prostate cancer screening tests. , 2000, The Journal of family practice.

[47]  Margaret Holmes-Rovner,et al.  Implementing shared decision‐making in routine practice: barriers and opportunities , 2000, Health expectations : an international journal of public participation in health care and health policy.

[48]  J G Thornton,et al.  A randomised trial of three methods of giving information about prenatal testing , 1995, BMJ.

[49]  Peter Tugwell,et al.  Randomized Trial of a Portable, Self-administered Decision Aid for Postmenopausal Women Considering Long-term Preventive Hormone Therapy , 1998, Medical decision making : an international journal of the Society for Medical Decision Making.