Electrophysiological correlates of second language processing
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Judith F. Kroll,et al. Accessing Conceptual Representations for Words in a Second Language , 1993 .
[2] Risto Näätänen,et al. Linguistic relevance of duration within the native language determines the accuracy of speech-sound duration processing. , 2003, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[3] Sonja A. Kotz,et al. Neurolinguistic evidence for bilingual language representation: a comparison of reaction times and event-related brain potentials , 2001 .
[4] Morris Halle,et al. The rules of language , 1980, IEEE Transactions on Professional Communication.
[5] D. Swinney,et al. Brain potentials elicited by garden-path sentences: evidence of the application of verb information during parsing. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[6] H. Neville,et al. Maturational Constraints on Functional Specializations for Language Processing: ERP and Behavioral Evidence in Bilingual Speakers , 1996, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[7] M. Huotilainen,et al. Event-related potentials associated with second language learning in children , 2003, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[8] M. Kutas,et al. Brain potentials during reading reflect word expectancy and semantic association , 1984, Nature.
[9] Paavo Alku,et al. Mismatch negativity shows that 3–6-year-old children can learn to discriminate non-native speech sounds within two months , 2002, Neuroscience Letters.
[10] Kerrie E. Elston-Güttler,et al. The role of proficiency on processing categorical and associative information in the L2 as revealed by reaction times and event-related brain potentials , 2004, Journal of Neurolinguistics.
[11] A. Friederici,et al. Event-related brain potentials during natural speech processing: effects of semantic, morphological and syntactic violations. , 1993, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[12] G. Yeni-Komshian,et al. Age Constraints on Second-Language Acquisition , 1999 .
[13] R. Näätänen. The perception of speech sounds by the human brain as reflected by the mismatch negativity (MMN) and its magnetic equivalent (MMNm). , 2001, Psychophysiology.
[14] Lisa D. Sanders,et al. An ERP study of continuous speech processing. II. Segmentation, semantics, and syntax in non-native speakers. , 2003, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[15] A Hahne,et al. What's Different in Second-Language Processing? Evidence from Event-Related Brain Potentials , 2001, Journal of psycholinguistic research.
[16] A. Friederici. Towards a neural basis of auditory sentence processing , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[17] M. Penke,et al. Brain potentials indicate differences between regular and irregular German plurals , 1997, Neuroreport.
[18] Margot J. Taylor,et al. Guidelines for using human event-related potentials to study cognition: recording standards and publication criteria. , 2000, Psychophysiology.
[19] H. Neville,et al. Language and , 2019, Adventure Diffusion.
[20] A. Friederici,et al. Processing a second language: Late learners''comprehension mechanisms as revealed by event-related b , 2001 .
[21] R. Ilmoniemi,et al. Language-specific phoneme representations revealed by electric and magnetic brain responses , 1997, Nature.
[22] E. Donchin. Presidential address, 1980. Surprise!...Surprise? , 1981, Psychophysiology.
[23] A. Hahne,et al. Second language learners' processing of inflected words: Behavioral and ERP evidence for storage and decomposition , 2003 .
[24] A. Friederici,et al. Electrophysiological Evidence for Two Steps in Syntactic Analysis: Early Automatic and Late Controlled Processes , 1999, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[25] M. Kutas,et al. An Event-Related Potential (ERP) Analysis of Semantic Congruity and Repetition Effects in Sentences , 1992, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[26] R. Ilmoniemi,et al. Brain responses reveal the learning of foreign language phonemes. , 1999, Psychophysiology.
[27] M Rugg,et al. Functional neuroimaging in cognitive neurosicence , 1999 .
[28] M. Rugg,et al. Electrophysiology of Mind: Event-Related Brain Potentials and Cognition , 1995 .
[29] David Birdsong. Ultimate attainment in second language acquisition , 1992 .
[30] M. Kutas,et al. Who Did What and When? Using Word- and Clause-Level ERPs to Monitor Working Memory Usage in Reading , 1995, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[31] Elissa L. Newport,et al. Segmenting nonsense: an event-related potential index of perceived onsets in continuous speech , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.
[32] Renata Meuter,et al. Brain responses to semantic incongruity in bilinguals , 1990, Brain and Language.
[33] M. Garrett,et al. Syntactically Based Sentence Processing Classes: Evidence from Event-Related Brain Potentials , 1991, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[34] M. Kutas,et al. Reading senseless sentences: brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. , 1980, Science.
[35] Michael T. Ullman,et al. The neural basis of lexicon and grammar in first and second language: the declarative/procedural model , 2001 .
[36] R. Näätänen,et al. Development of language-specific phoneme representations in the infant brain , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.
[37] Laura Sabourin. Grammatical Gender and Second Language Processing , 2003 .
[38] H. Neville,et al. Sensitive periods differentiate processing of open- and closed-class words: an ERP study of bilinguals. , 2001, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.
[39] Lisa D. Sanders,et al. An ERP study of continuous speech processing. I. Segmentation, semantics, and syntax in native speakers. , 2003, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[40] T. Dijkstra,et al. Language context effects on interlingual homograph recognition: Evidence from event-related potentials and response times in semantic priming , 2001 .
[41] E. Gibson,et al. The P600 as an index of syntactic integration difficulty , 2000 .
[42] J. Kroll,et al. Category Interference in Translation and Picture Naming: Evidence for Asymmetric Connections Between Bilingual Memory Representations , 1994 .
[43] Karsten Steinhauer,et al. Brain signatures of artificial language processing: Evidence challenging the critical period hypothesis , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[44] M. Kutas,et al. Expect the Unexpected: Event-related Brain Response to Morphosyntactic Violations , 1998 .
[45] J. S. Johnson,et al. Critical period effects in second language learning: The influence of maturational state on the acquisition of English as a second language , 1989, Cognitive Psychology.
[46] Cheryl Frenck-Mestre,et al. Second Language Autonomy , 1997 .
[47] H. Neville,et al. Fractionating language: different neural subsystems with different sensitive periods. , 1992, Cerebral cortex.
[48] J. H. Neely. Semantic priming effects in visual word recognition: A selective review of current findings and theories. , 1991 .
[49] Kara D. Federmeier,et al. Electrophysiology reveals semantic memory use in language comprehension , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[50] C. C. Wood,et al. Event-related potentials, lexical decision and semantic priming. , 1985, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.