Inducing Task-Relevant Responses to Speech in the Sleeping Brain

Summary Falling asleep leads to a loss of sensory awareness and to the inability to interact with the environment [1]. While this was traditionally thought as a consequence of the brain shutting down to external inputs, it is now acknowledged that incoming stimuli can still be processed, at least to some extent, during sleep [2]. For instance, sleeping participants can create novel sensory associations between tones and odors [3] or reactivate existing semantic associations, as evidenced by event-related potentials [4, 5, 6, 7]. Yet, the extent to which the brain continues to process external stimuli remains largely unknown. In particular, it remains unclear whether sensory information can be processed in a flexible and task-dependent manner by the sleeping brain, all the way up to the preparation of relevant actions. Here, using semantic categorization and lexical decision tasks, we studied task-relevant responses triggered by spoken stimuli in the sleeping brain. Awake participants classified words as either animals or objects (experiment 1) or as either words or pseudowords (experiment 2) by pressing a button with their right or left hand, while transitioning toward sleep. The lateralized readiness potential (LRP), an electrophysiological index of response preparation, revealed that task-specific preparatory responses are preserved during sleep. These findings demonstrate that despite the absence of awareness and behavioral responsiveness, sleepers can still extract task-relevant information from external stimuli and covertly prepare for appropriate motor responses.

[1]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Levels of processing during non-conscious perception: a critical review of visual masking , 2007, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[2]  Dragan Rangelov,et al.  How the speed of motor-response decisions, but not focal-attentional selection, differs as a function of task set and target prevalence , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[3]  Emmanuel Dupoux,et al.  Subliminal Speech Priming , 2005, Psychological science.

[4]  P. Haggard Conscious intention and motor cognition , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[5]  J. Hobson,et al.  The prefrontal cortex in sleep , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[6]  K. Ruddock Blindsight: a case study and implications. Oxford Psychology Series No 12. , 1987 .

[7]  Peter Praamstra,et al.  Frontoparietal control of spatial attention and motor intention in human EEG. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[8]  M. Vidailhet,et al.  Evidence for the Re-Enactment of a Recently Learned Behavior during Sleepwalking , 2011, PloS one.

[9]  L. Weiskrantz Blindsight : a case study and implications , 1986 .

[10]  Maquet,et al.  Functional neuroimaging of normal human sleep by positron emission tomography , 2000, Journal of sleep research.

[11]  Srivas Chennu,et al.  Arousal Modulates Auditory Attention and Awareness: Insights from Sleep, Sedation, and Disorders of Consciousness , 2011, Front. Psychology.

[12]  Corinne A. Bareham,et al.  Losing the left side of the world: Rightward shift in human spatial attention with sleep onset , 2014, Scientific Reports.

[13]  N. Sobel,et al.  Humans can learn new information during sleep , 2012, Nature Neuroscience.

[14]  Marc Brysbaert,et al.  Lexique 2 : A new French lexical database , 2004, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.

[15]  A. M. Taylor,et al.  Discriminative responses to stimulation during human sleep. , 1960, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[16]  Fabien Perrin,et al.  Semantic analysis of auditory input during sleep: studies with event related potentials. , 2002, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[17]  F. Morales,et al.  Chapter 12 – Control of Motoneurons during Sleep , 2005 .

[18]  Alan Cowey,et al.  Stimulus cueing in blindsight. , 2004, Progress in brain research.

[19]  G. Tononi,et al.  Breakdown of Cortical Effective Connectivity During Sleep , 2005, Science.

[20]  R. Oostenveld,et al.  Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG- and MEG-data , 2007, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[21]  H. Leuthold Programming of expected and unexpected movements: effects on the onset of the lateralized readiness potential. , 2003, Acta psychologica.

[22]  F. Mauguière,et al.  Thalamic deactivation at sleep onset precedes that of the cerebral cortex in humans , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[23]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Imaging unconscious semantic priming , 1998, Nature.

[24]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[25]  R. Ogilvie The process of falling asleep. , 2001, Sleep medicine reviews.

[26]  M. Eimer,et al.  Effects of masked stimuli on motor activation: behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[27]  D Formby,et al.  Maternal Recognition of Infant's Cry , 1967, Developmental medicine and child neurology.

[28]  Elias B. Issa,et al.  Sensory Responses during Sleep in Primate Primary and Secondary Auditory Cortex , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[29]  J. Edeline,et al.  Neural representations during sleep: From sensory processing to memory traces , 2007, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[30]  J. Valdizán,et al.  Auditory event-related potentials to semantic priming during sleep. , 1998, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[31]  C. Loeb,et al.  Hypnic physiological myoclonias in man: an EEG-EMG study in normals and neurological patients. , 1969, European neurology.

[32]  T Hori,et al.  Topographical characteristics and principal component structure of the hypnagogic EEG. , 1997, Sleep.

[33]  M. Coles Modern mind-brain reading: psychophysiology, physiology, and cognition. , 1989, Psychophysiology.

[34]  Vladimir López,et al.  ERPs and contextual semantic discrimination: Degrees of congruence in wakefulness and sleep , 2006, Brain and Language.

[35]  E. Donchin,et al.  Preparation to respond as manifested by movement-related brain potentials , 1980, Brain Research.

[36]  I. Thomas,et al.  How does the pitch and pattern of a signal affect auditory arousal thresholds? , 2009, Journal of sleep research.

[37]  Tadao Hori,et al.  Topographical EEG changes and the hypnagogic experience. , 1994 .

[38]  B. Baars A cognitive theory of consciousness , 1988 .

[39]  T. Bekinschtein,et al.  Cognitive processing during the transition to sleep. , 2012, Archives italiennes de biologie.

[40]  A. Chesson,et al.  The AASM Manual for the Scoring of Sleep and Associated Events: Rules, Terminology, and Techinical Specifications , 2007 .

[41]  J. Armony,et al.  Auditory Processing across the Sleep-Wake Cycle Simultaneous EEG and fMRI Monitoring in Humans , 2000, Neuron.

[42]  A. Chesson,et al.  The American Academy of Sleep Medicine (AASM) Manual for the Scoring of Sleep and Associated Events: Rules, Terminology and Technical Specifications , 2007 .

[43]  W. Klimesch,et al.  'Paradoxical' alpha synchronization in a memory task. , 1999, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[44]  J. Schwarzbach,et al.  Different time courses for visual perception and action priming , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[45]  Hiroaki Masaki,et al.  The functional locus of the lateralized readiness potential. , 2004, Psychophysiology.

[46]  G. Tononi,et al.  Auditory responses and stimulus-specific adaptation in rat auditory cortex are preserved across NREM and REM sleep. , 2015, Cerebral cortex.